PSYC 421 Exam 2

PSYC 421 Exam 2

IMPORTANT: AFTER PURCHASE, LOG IN TO YOUR ACCOUNT AND SCROLL DOWN BELOW THIS PAGE TO DOWNLOAD FILES WITH ANSWERS.

1. Factor analysis can help the test developer

2. As the result of a sensitivity review, items containing ________ may be eliminated from a test.

3. When an item-characteristic curve of an ability test has an inverted U shape, it usually indicates that

4. Ability tests are typically standardized on a sample that is representative of the general population and selected on the basis of variables such as

5. Which is a major difference between multiple-choice questions and essay questions?

6. The best type of item yields an item-characteristic curve that

7. Looking at the item-characteristic curve (below), a reasonable conclusion about the performance of the item illustrated would be that

8. If all raw scores on a test are to be converted to scores that range only from 1 to 9, the resulting scale is referred to as this type of scale:

9. The reason latent-trait theory is so-named has to do with the presumption that

10. In item analysis, the term item endorsement refers to the percent of testtakers who

11. Brotto and Yule reported that the development of their measure of asexuality was developed in four stages. Which best characterizes what they did during Stages 2 and 3?

12. A test developer of multiple-choice ability tests reviews data from a recent test administration. She discovers that testtakers who scored very high on the test as a whole, all responded to item 13 with the same incorrect choice. Accordingly, the test developer

13. The concept of asexuality was first introduced by

14. The development of a criterion-referenced test usually entails

15. An individually administered designed for use with elementary-school-age student is in the test tryout stage of test development. For the purposes of the tryout, this test should be administered

16. An item-characteristic curve includes all of the following EXCEPT

17. According to Brotto and Yule, their new measure of asexuality performed satisfactorily on

18. Which is TRUE of the latent-trait model of measurement?

19. These tests are often used for the purpose of licensing persons in professions. The tests referred to here are

20. A “good” test item on an ability test is one

21. Estimates suggest that approximately ________% of the population might be asexual.

22. In Guttman scaling

23. Generous time limits are typically associated with

24. According to your textbook, the minimum sample for a test tryout is

25. Scoring drift refers to

BUY MORE MATERIALS FOR THIS COURSE:

PSYC 421 Quiz 1

PSYC 421 Quiz 2

PSYC 421 Quiz 3

PSYC 421 Quiz 4

PSYC 421 Quiz 5

PSYC 421 Quiz 6

PSYC 421 Quiz 7

PSYC 421 Exam 1

PSYC 421 Exam 2

PSYC 421 Exam 3

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology Discussion homework help

Psychology Discussion homework help

Chapter 10

One of the most important and widely studied areas in cultural psychology is personality. Indeed, the search for the underlying bases of individual differences, which serve as the backbone of understanding personality, shares a close conceptual and empirical connection with culture in any cultural milieu. We begin this chapter by first defining personality, discussing briefly the major perspectives that have been used to study it, and the measurement of personality across cultures. Then we review cross-cultural research on a view of personality known as the Five-Factor Model (FFM), which suggests that five personality dimensions are universal to all humans. We discuss two theories that account for such universality in personality structure and research that goes beyond the FFM. We also discuss indigenous and culture-specific approaches to personality and some of the research that has been conducted in this area. Although culture-specific aspects of personality and universal notions of personality may seem contradictory, we present a way of understanding their mutual coexistence and conceptualizing and studying their duality.

DEFINING PERSONALITY

Definitions

Personality  is a broad concept that refers to many aspects of an individual’s unique characteristics, and is generally considered to be a set of relatively enduring behavioral and cognitive characteristics, traits, or predispositions that people take with them to different situations, contexts, and interactions with others, and that contribute to differences among individuals. They are the qualities or collection of qualities that make a person a distinctive individual, or the collective aggregate of behavioral and mental characteristics that are distinctive of an individual. Personality is generally believed to be relatively stable across time and consistent across contexts, situations, and interactions (Allport,  1936 ; Funder,  2001 ).

Over the years, scientists have identified and studied many specific aspects of personality within this broad definition, and we believe that it’s helpful to understand the broad concept of personality along multiple levels of analysis. In this chapter, we broadly define personality along two broad levels of analysis, which allows us to understand potentially disparate approaches to the study and understanding of personality across cultures. One level includes what are known as dispositional traits, or just traits for short. A  trait  is a characteristic or quality distinguishing a person. It refers to a consistent pattern of behavior, feelings, and thoughts that a person would usually display in relevant circumstances. For example, if we describe someone as “outgoing,” that would generally refer to a specific pattern of behavior in which this person is likely to engage. A person who is outgoing will likely strike up conversations, meet comfortably with strangers, and be expressive. A person who is “shy” would not. The trait approach in psychology has a long and rich history, dating to the work of Allport ( 1936 ). Theories and research on this area of personality are known as trait psychology.

Another level of personality can be broadly construed as  identity , which would include our perceived roles in life, aggregate role and life experiences, narratives, values, and motives (Markus & Kitayama,  1998 ; Wood & Roberts,  2006 ). These aspects of our personalities are created by performing repeated roles—thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that occur in real life across single role experiences—producing a history that comprises aggregate role experiences. These experiences, in turn, form the basis of other important aspects of personality, including narratives, values, and general motives (Roberts,  2006 ).

Perspectives

Some of the earliest contributions to our understanding of the relationship between personality and culture came from anthropologists who were interested in psychology. Through mostly ethnographic fieldwork, anthropologists such as Margaret Mead, Edward Sapir, Weston Labarre, Ruth Benedict, Ralph Linton, Cora DuBois, and Abraham Kardiner developed theories about culture and personality that served as a basis for cross-cultural comparison of personalities and today’s cultural psychology (see review in Piker,  1998 ). Many of these works formed the basis for the notion of “national character,” which is still popular today. A  national character  refers to the perception that each culture has a modal personality type, and that most persons in that culture share aspects of it. Although many cultural and psychological anthropologists recognize the important contributions of biologically innate factors to personality and psychology, the main thrust of the anthropological contribution is its view of personality as culturally specific, formed by the unique forces each culture deals with in its milieu. The anthropological view of personality, therefore, attributes more importance to the learning of psychological mechanisms and personality in the environment through cultural practices than to biological and evolutionary factors. It was believed that foundations of personality development were set in early childhood according to each culture’s unique cultural traits.

Whereas psychological anthropology made major contributions to the study of culture and personality in the first half of the 20th century, the second half was dominated by cross-cultural psychological research, which focused on traits (see review by Church & Lonner,  1998 ). This approach generally views personality as something discrete and separate from culture, and as a dependent variable in research. Thus, two or more cultures are treated as independent variables, and they are compared on some personality traits or dimensions. In contrast to the cultural or psychological anthropological approach, the cross-cultural approach tends to see personality as an etic or universal phenomenon that is equivalently relevant and meaningful in the cultures being compared. To the extent that personality does exhibit universal aspects, how did they originate?

Cross-cultural research on personality, however, has also been concerned with the discovery of culture-specific personality traits. Cross-cultural psychologists describe culture-specific  indigenous personalities  as constellations of personality traits and characteristics found only in a specific culture (for more information, see reviews by Ho,  1998 ; Diaz-Loving,  1998 ). These types of studies, though psychological in nature, are heavily influenced in approach and understanding by the anthropological view of culture and personality.

Work on indigenous personalities has led to what is known as the cultural perspective to personality (for example, Shweder,  1979a  1979b  1980  1991 ; Markus & Kitayama,  1998 ). This approach sees culture and personality not as separate entities, but as a mutually constituted system in which each creates and maintains the other.

· The cultural perspective assumes that psychological processes, in this case the nature of functioning of personality, are not just influenced by culture but are thoroughly culturally constituted. In turn, the cultural perspective assumes that personalities behaving in concert create the culture. Culture and personality are most productively analyzed together as a dynamic of mutual constitution …; one cannot be reduced to the other.… A cultural psychological approach does not automatically assume that all behavior can be explained with the same set of categories and dimensions and first asks whether a given dimension, concept, or category is meaningful and how it is used in a given cultural context. (Markus & Kitayama,  1998 , p. 66)

The cultural perspective has been heavily influenced by the cultural anthropologists, as well as by the cross-cultural work on indigenous psychologies (see Kim,  2001 ) and personalities. On its face, it is somewhat antithetical to the crosscultural search for universals and rejects the possibility of biological and genetic mechanisms underlying universality. Instead, it suggests that just as no two cultures are alike, the personalities that comprise those cultures should be fundamentally different because of the mutual constitution of culture and personality within each cultural milieu.

Thus today, there are two major perspectives in cultural psychology with regard to understanding personality. One perspective, rooted in the study of traits, suggests that personality organization and dimensions are universal (and somewhat biologically innate, as we will see below). The other perspective, rooted in indigenous, cultural perspectives of personality as identities, suggests that personalities are dependent on the cultures in which they exist, and rejects notions of universality. How to make sense of this all is perhaps the greatest challenge facing this area of cultural psychology in the near future. Below we will review some of the major research evidence for both perspectives, and describe an integrated perspective that suggests that the universal and indigenous approaches are not necessarily mutually exclusive to each other. This later analysis will also make use of the understanding of different levels of personality that we described earlier.

Measuring Personality across Cultures

Before delving into what we know in this area, we need to contend with one of the most serious issues in all cross-cultural research on personality: whether personality can be measured reliably and validly across different cultures. If methods of assessing personality are not reliable or valid across cultures, then the results of research using these methods cannot be trusted to give accurate portrayals of personality similarities or differences across cultures.

This issue is directly related to the differences in perspectives discussed immediately above. The etic, universal perspective to personality assumes, for instance, that there are aspects of personality that exist across cultures, that they can be measured in similar ways across cultures, and that the results of those measurements can be compared across cultures. The emic, indigenous perspective, however, would suggest that because aspects of personality are likely to be culture-specific, it is difficult if not impossible to create measures of personality that have the same meaning (and validity) across cultures. Thus, when considering the measurement of personality across cultures, we need to first consider the aspect of personality that is being measured and the theoretical perspective of the researcher measuring it.

If one assumes that there are aspects of personality that can be measured and compared across cultures, then important questions arise concerning its measurement. Most personality measures used in cross-cultural research were originally developed in a single language and single culture and validated in that language and culture. The psychometric evidence typically used to demonstrate a measure’s reliability and validity in a single culture involves examination of internal, testretest, and parallel forms reliabilities, convergent and predictive validities, and replicability of the factor structures that comprise the various scales of the test. To obtain all these types of psychometric evidence for the reliability and validity of a test, researchers must literally spend years conducting countless studies addressing each of these specific concerns. The best measures of personality—as well as all other psychological constructs—have this degree of psychometric evidence backing them.

A common practice in many of the early cross-cultural studies on personality was to take a personality scale that had been developed in one country or culture—most often the United States—and simply translate it and use it in another culture. In effect, the researchers simply assumed that the personality dimension measured by that scale was equivalent between the two cultures, and that the method of measuring that dimension was psychometrically valid and reliable. Thus, many studies imposed an assumed etic construct upon the cultures studied (Church & Lonner, 1998 ). Realistically, however, one cannot safely conclude that the personality dimensions represented by an imposed etic are equivalently and meaningfully represented in all cultures included in a study.

The mere fact that personality scales have been translated and used in crosscultural research is not sufficient evidence that the personality domains they measure are indeed equivalent in those cultures. In fact, when this type of research is conducted, one of the researchers’ primary concerns is whether the personality scales used in the study can validly and reliably measure meaningful dimensions of personality in all the cultures studied. As discussed in  Chapter 2 , the equivalence of a measure in terms of its meaning to all cultures concerned, as well as its psychometric validity and reliability, is of prime concern in cross-cultural research if the results are to be considered valid, meaningful, and useful.

The cross-cultural validation of personality measures requires psychometric evidence from all cultures in which the test is to be used. In the strictest sense, therefore, researchers interested in cross-cultural studies on personality should select instruments that have been demonstrated to have acceptable psychometric properties in cultures of interest. This is a far cry from merely selecting a test that seems to be interesting and translating it for use in another culture. At the very least, equivalence of its psychometric properties should be established empirically, not assumed or ignored (Matsumoto & Van de Vijver,  2011 ).

Data addressing the psychometric evidence necessary to validate a test in a target culture would provide the safest avenue by which such equivalence can be demonstrated. If such data exist, they can be used to support contentions concerning psychometric equivalence. Even if those data do not offer a high degree of support (reliability coefficients are lower, or factor structures are not exactly equivalent), that does not necessarily mean that the test as a whole is not equivalent. There are, in fact, multiple alternative explanations of why such data may not be as strong in the target culture as in the culture in which the test was originally developed. Paunonen and Ashton ( 1998 ) outline and describe ten such possible interpretations, ranging from poor test translation and response style issues to different analytic methods. Thus, if a test is examined in another culture for its psychometric properties and the data are not as strong as they were in the original culture, each of these possibilities should be examined before concluding that the test is not psychometrically valid or reliable. In many cases, the problem may be minor and fixable.

Fortunately, many of the more recent studies in this area have been sensitive to this issue, and researchers have taken steps to ensure some degree of psychometric equivalence across cultures in their measures of personality. Tests assessing traits have a long history in cross-cultural research, and researchers have addressed issues of cross-cultural equivalence and validity of their measures for years. The NEO PI-R, for example, and its subsequent NEO PI3, which was used in many of the studies described below on traits, has undergone extensive cross-cultural reliability, validity, and equivalence testing (Costa & McCrae,  1992 ; McCrae, Costa, & Martin,  2005 ). Similar findings have been obtained using other tests of traits, such as the California Psychological Inventory, the Comrey Personality Scales, the 16 Personality Factors Questionnaire, the Pavlovian Temperament Survey, the Personality Research Form, and the Nonverbal Personality Questionnaire (Paunonen & Ashton, 1998 ). Studies demonstrating the relationship between traits and adjustment, and the possible biological sources of traits (reviewed below), also lend support to the cross-cultural validity of the measures. Thus, the research findings we report below concerning traits and other personality dimensions have used measures that appear to be equivalent and valid across cultures.

CROSS-CULTURAL STUDIES ON PERSONALITY TRAITS: THE FIVE-FACTOR MODEL

Evidence for the Five-Factor Model

In the past two decades, trait approaches to personality have become extremely important in understanding the relationship between culture and personality, and it is the dominant view today. This work has culminated in what is known today as the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality, which we now describe.

The FFM is a conceptual model built around five distinct and basic personality dimensions that appear to be universal for all humans. The five dimensions are neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The FFM was conceived after a number of researchers noticed the similarities in the personality dimensions that had emerged across many studies, both within and between cultures. Most notably, support for the FFM arose out of factor analyses of trait adjectives from the English lexicon that were descriptive of self and others (Juni, 1996 ). The factors that emerged from these types of analyses were similar to dimensions found in the analysis of questionnaire scales operationalizing personality. Further inquiry across cultures, using both factor analysis of descriptive trait adjectives in different languages and personality dimensions measured by different personality questionnaires, lent further credence to the FFM.

Many early (e.g., Eysenck’s,  1983 ) and contemporary studies have provided support for the cross-cultural validity of the FFM, spanning different countries and cultures in Europe, East and South Asia, North America, Africa, and Australia. One of the leading researchers on personality and culture in the tradition of the FFM is Robert R. McCrae, who published self-report data for 26 countries in 2001 (McCrae,  2001 ). In 2002, the database was expanded to 36 cultures (Allik & McCrae,  2004 ; McCrae,  2002 ). In one of the latest studies in this line of work, McCrae and his colleagues in 51 cultures of the world replicated the FFM in all cultures studied (McCrae, Terracciano, Khoury, et al.,  2005 ; McCrae, Terracciano, Leibovich, et al.,  2005 ). Collectively, these studies provide convincing and substantial evidence to support the claim that the FFM—consisting of neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness—represents a universal taxonomy of personality that is applicable to all humans.

One of the most widely used measures of the FFM in previous research was the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) (Costa & McCrae,  1992 ), which has been revised as the NEO PI-3 (McCrae, Costa, & Martin,  2005 ). It is a 240-item instrument in which respondents rate the degree to which they agree or disagree that the item is characteristic of them. These instruments have been used in many studies across many different cultures. It produces scores on the five major personality traits, as well as six subscores for each major trait ( Table 10.1 ).

Two of the most important traits for describing behavioral differences are extraversion and neuroticism. The former refers to the degree to which an individual experiences positive emotions, and is outgoing, expressive, and sociable or shy, introverted, and avoids contact; the latter refers to the degree of emotional stability in an individual. McCrae, Terracciano, Khoury et al. ( 2005 ) graphed the cultural groups they studied along these two dimensions in order to create a useful visual aid in distinguishing among the cultures in terms of their personality ( Figure 10.1 ). Examining this graph provides some ideas about the average personality traits of individuals in these cultural groups. Americans, New Zealanders, and Australians, for instance, tend to be high on extraversion and in the middle of the scale for neuroticism.

One of the concerns with findings generated with scales like the NEO PI-R is that the findings may reflect bias on the part of the respondent to answer in a socially desirable way (see  Chapter 2  to review response biases). These concerns are especially noted in cross-cultural work. McCrae, Terracciano, Leibovich, and colleagues ( 2005 ), therefore, conducted a follow-up study in which they asked samples of adults and college students in 50 cultural groups to rate someone they know well on the NEO PI-R. The questionnaire was modified so that the ratings were done in the third person. Analyses revealed that the same five-factor model emerged, indicating that the previous results were not dependent on ratings of oneself. In another interesting study, Allik and McCrae ( 2004 ) showed that the personality traits were not related to geographic location (defined as distance from the equator or mean temperature); but, geographically or historically close cultures had more similar personality profiles. Collectively, the results to date provide strong evidence that the FFM is a universal model of personality structure.

Table 10.1 Traits Associated with the Five-Factor Model

Major Trait Subtrait
Neuroticism Anxiety

Angry hostility

Depression

Self-consciousness

Impulsiveness

Vulnerability

Extraversion Warmth

Gregariousness

Assertiveness

Activity

Excitement seeking

Positive emotions

Openness Fantasy

Aesthetics

Feelings

Actions

Ideas

Values

Agreeableness Trust

Straightforwardness

Altruism

Compliance

Modesty

Tender-mindedness

Conscientiousness Competence

Order

Dutifulness

Achievement striving

Self-discipline

Deliberation

Figure 10.1 Graphic Display of Cultures from McCrae et al. ( 2005 )

 

The vertical axis refers to Neuroticism, while the horizontal axis refers to Extroversion. HK Chinese = Hong Kong Chinese; N. Irish = Northern Irish; S. Koreans = South Koreans.

Source: McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., Leibovich, N. B., Schmidt, V., Shakespeare-Finch, J., Neubauer, A., et al., “Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate personality traits,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, pp. 407-425, 2005, Copyright © American Psychological Association. Reprinted by permission.

Do Perceptions of National Character Correspond to Aggregate Personality Traits?

The works by McCrae and others described above have been important because they have measured the actual personality traits of large numbers of individuals in a wide range of cultures. Thus, they are reliable data on what the actual personalities of individuals in these cultures are like. One of the things that these data allows us to do is to compare those actual personality profiles with our perceptions of national character. As described above, national character refers to perceptions of the average personality of people of different cultures. Perceptions of national character are, in fact, stereotypes about average personalities of people of different cultures.

But are they accurate? Terraciano et al. (2005) asked approximately 4,000 respondents in 49 cultures to describe the “typical member” of a culture using 30 bipolar scales with two or three trait adjectives on the poles of each scale. They found that there was relatively high agreement about the national character perceptions of the various cultures; but, these perceptions were not correlated with the actual personality trait levels of the individuals of those very same cultures. In other words, perceptions of national character were not correlated with the actual, aggregate personality levels of individuals of those cultures. One of the limitations of that study, moreover, was that different measures were used to assess personality and national character. Two subsequent studies corrected for this limitation, and found some degree of similarity between the two ratings, but with considerable dissimilarity as well (Allik, Mottus, & Realo,  2010 ; Realo et al.,  2009 ). These findings suggested that perceptions of national character may actually be unfounded stereotypes of the personalities of members of those cultures to some degree.

If perceptions of national character are inaccurate, why do we have them? Terraciano and colleagues (2005) suggested that one of the functions of these unfounded stereotypes is the maintenance of a national identity. That is, one of the functions of stereotypes about other groups is to affirm, or reaffirm, the perceptions, and often the self-worth, of one’s own group. Sometimes, these functions are dangerous; when perceptions of others are unfavorable, they often lead to prejudice, discrimination, and violence. Other sources of personality stereotypes may be climate, national wealth, values, or social desirability (Allik, et al.,  2010 ; McCrae, Terracciano, Realo, & Allik, 2007 ; Realo, et al.,  2009 ).

Where Do these Traits Come From? The Five-Factor Theory

It is important to distinguish between the Five-Factor Model (FFM) of personality, which is a model of the number of traits that are universal to all people in their personality structure, and the Five-Factor Theory (FFT) of personality, which is a theory about the source of those traits. One is not entirely dependent on the other; the model of the traits may be entirely correct, while the theory about where they come from entirely wrong. Alternatively, research may show that there are more than five universal traits, while the theory that explains them is correct. Here we discuss the FFT, which attempts to account for where the universal personality traits come from.

The major proponents of the FFT are, not surprisingly, McCrae and Costa ( 1999 ). According to them, the core components of the FFT are Basic Tendencies, Characteristic Adaptations, and the Self-Concept, which is actually a subcomponent of Characteristic Adaptations.

The traits correspond to the Basic Tendencies; they refer to internal dispositions to respond to the environment in certain, predictable ways. The FFT suggests that personality traits that underlie basic tendencies are biologically based. Several sources of evidence support this idea. As described earlier, the same personality traits have been found in all cultures studied, and using different research methods (McCrae, Terracciano, Khoury, Nansubuga, Knezevic, Djuric Jocic et al.,  2005 ; McCrae et al.,  2005 ). Parent-child relationships have little lasting effect on personality traits (Rowe, 1994 ); and traits are generally stable across the adult lifespan (McCrae & Costa,  2003 ), although there are some developmental changes (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer,  2006 ). Studies of twins demonstrate that the personalities of identical twins reared apart are much more similar than those of fraternal twins reared together (Bouchard & Loehlin,  2001 ; Bouchard, Lykken, & McGue, 1994 ). The FFM can predict variations in behavior among individuals in longitudinal studies (Borkenau & Ostendorf,  1998 ), and some evidence suggests that the FFM may apply to nonhuman primates as well (King & Figueredo,  1997 ).

The FFT suggests that the universal personality traits representing basic tendencies are expressed in characteristic ways; these characteristic ways can be largely influenced by the culture in which one exists, and here is where culture has important influences on personality development and expression. Characteristic Adaptations include habits, attitudes, skills, roles, and relationships. They are characteristic because they reflect the psychological core personality trait dispositions of the individual; they are also adaptations because they help the individual fit into the ever-changing social environment (McCrae & Costa,  1999 ). Culture can substantially influence these characteristic adaptations through the resources, social structures, and social systems available in a specific environment to help achieve goals. Culture can influence values about the various personality traits. Culture defines context and provides differential meaning to the components of context, including who is involved, what is happening, where it is occurring, and the like. Culture, therefore, plays a substantial role in producing the specific behavioral manifestations—the specific action units—that individuals will engage in to achieve what may be universal affective goals. Culture is “undeniably relevant in the development of characteristics and adaptations that guide the expression of personality in thoughts, feelings, and behaviors” (McCrae et al.,  1998 ), and the characteristic adaptations vary greatly across cultures. The Basic Tendencies representing the universal personality traits, however, are not culturally variable, and a universal personality structure is the mechanism by which such goals are achieved through a balance and interaction with culture.

The characteristic adaptations help to produce a self-concept, as well as specific behaviors. For example, a person low in Depression, a facet of Neuroticism (Basic Tendency), may develop a low self-esteem, irrational perfectionistic beliefs, and pessimistic or cynical attitudes about the world (Characteristic Adaptations and Self-Concept). He or she may thus feel guilty about work or unsatisfied with his or her life (behavior). A person high on Gregariousness, however, which is part of Extraversion (Basic Tendency), may be outgoing, friendly, and talkative (Characteristic Adaptations). This person is likely to have numerous friendships and be a member of various social clubs (behaviors).

To be sure, one of the most contentious parts of the FFT is its suggestion that the origin of the personality traits are almost entirely, if not entirely, biologically determined. An alternative perspective suggests a role of culture or environment in the shaping of the personality traits underlying Basic Tendencies of behavior (Allik & McCrae,  2002 ; Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt,  2003 ; Roberts & Helson,  1997 ; Roberts, Helson, & Klohnen,  2002 ). There is little debate that culture caninfluence the Characteristic Adaptations and Self-Concepts associated with underlying personality traits (Heine & Buchtel,  2009 ). Debate continues concerning the origins of the traits, and future research in this area will undoubtedly need to explore many possibilities.

An Evolutionary Approach

To explain the universality of the FFM, some (for example, MacDonald,  1998 ) have suggested an evolutionary approach. This approach posits universality both of human interests and of the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying trait variation. Personality structure is viewed as a universal psychological mechanism, a product of natural selection that serves both social and nonsocial functions in problem solving and environmental adaptation. Based on this theory, one would expect to find similar systems in animals that serve similar adaptive functions, and one would expect personality systems to be organized within the brain as discrete neurophysiological systems. One of the key questions about the FFM that an evolutionary perspective brings, for example, concerns why socially undesirable traits like Neuroticism have been preserved through evolution (Penke, Denissen, & Miller,  2007 ).

In the evolutionary view, traits such as Conscientiousness (which refers to the degree of organization, persistence, control, and motivation in goal-directed behavior), Neuroticism (tendency to experience negative emotions, vulnerability to stress, emotional stability), and the other components of the FFM are considered to reflect stable variations in systems that serve critical adaptive functions. Conscientiousness, for example, may help individuals to monitor the environment for dangers and impending punishments, and to persevere in tasks that are not intrinsically rewarding (MacDonald,  1998 ). Neuroticism may be adaptive because it helps mobilize behavioral resources by moderating arousal in situations requiring approach or avoidance.

According to MacDonald ( 1991  1998 ), this evolutionary approach suggests a hierarchical model in which “behavior related to personality occurs at several levels based ultimately on the motivating aspects of evolved personality systems” (p. 130). In this model, humans possess evolved motive dispositions—for example, intimacy, safety—that are serviced by a universal set of personality dispositions that help individuals achieve their affective goals by managing personal and environmental resources. This resource management leads to concerns, projects, and tasks, which in turn lead to specific action units or behaviors through which the individual achieves the goals specified by the evolved motive dispositions (see  Figure 10.2 ).

Note that this model—and the assumptions about universality of the FFM made by McCrae and Costa and others (for example, McCrae & Costa,  1997 )—does not minimize the importance of cultural and individual variability. Culture can substantially influence personality through the resources, social structures, and social systems available in a specific environment to help achieve goals. Culture can therefore influence mean levels of personality and values about the various personality traits. As stated earlier, culture is “undeniably relevant in the development of characteristics and adaptations that guide the expression of personality in thoughts, feelings, and behaviors” (McCrae et al.,  1998 ). Culture defines context and provides differential meaning to the components of context, including who is involved, what is happening, where it is occurring, and the like. Culture, therefore, plays a substantial role in producing the specific behavioral manifestations—the specific action units—that individuals will engage in to achieve what may be universal affective goals. A universal personality structure, however, is considered to be the mechanism by which such goals are achieved through a balance and interaction with culture.

Figure 10.2 Hierarchical Model of Motivation Showing Relationships Between Domain-Specific and Domain-General Mechanisms

 

Source: Republished with permission of Taylor & Francis Group LLC—Books, from Goal concepts in personality and social psychology, Pervin, L (Ed.), 1989. Permission conveyed through Copyright Clearance Center, Inc.

CROSS-CULTURAL STUDIES ON OTHER DIMENSIONS OF PERSONALITY

Are There More than Five Universal Traits?

Research documenting the robustness of the FFM of personality traits around the world has clearly made a major contribution to our understanding of personality organization and culture. Still, there are several lines of research that challenge whether five factors are enough. One of these challenges is that, because the FFM was essentially created in the United States by American researchers, it may be the case that its measurement is missing other important factors not intended to be measured in the first place.

Interpersonal Relatedness

One important line of research has been led by Fanny Cheung and colleagues (2001). They began their work with the idea that the FFM might be missing some important features of personality in Asia, and specifically China. Specifically, they thought that none of the FFM traits dealt well with issues of relationships, which are central in China (as well as many cultures around the world). Thus, they developed what they initially considered an indigenous scale designed to measure personality in China that included the following traits:

· • Harmony, which refers to one’s inner peace of mind, contentment, interpersonal harmony, avoidance of conflict, and maintenance of equilibrium;

· • Ren Qing (relationship orientation), which covers adherence to cultural norms of interaction based on reciprocity, exchange of social favors, and exchange of affection according to implicit rules;

· • Modernization, which is reflected by personality change in response to societal modernization and attitudes toward traditional Chinese beliefs;

· • Thrift vs. Extravagance, which highlights the traditional virtue of saving rather than wasting and carefulness in spending, in contrast to the willingness to spend money for hedonistic purposes;

· • Ah-Q Mentality (defensiveness), which is based on a character in a popular Chinese novel in which the defense mechanisms of the Chinese people, including self-protective rationalization, externationalization of blame, and belittling of others’ achievements, are satirized;

· • Face, which depicts the pattern of orientations in an international and hierarchical connection and social behaviors to enhance one’s face and to avoid losing one’s face (Cheung, Leung, Zhang, Sun, Gan, Song et al.,  2001 ) (p. 408).

Collectively, Cheung and colleagues have named these dimensions “Interpersonal Relatedness.” Although they originally found support for the existence of this dimension in their studies of mainland and Hong Kong Chinese, they have also created an English version of their scale and documented the existence of the Interpersonal Relatedness dimension in samples from Singapore, Hawaii, the Midwestern United States, and with Chinese and European Americans (Cheung, Cheung, Leung, Ward, & Leong,  2003 ; Cheung et al.,  2001 ; Lin & Church,  2004 ).

Filipino Personality Structure

Another major line of research that challenges whether the FFM is enough comes from studies on the personality structures of Filipinos headed by Church and colleagues. In early research, they identified as many traits as they could that existed in the Filipino language, and asked Filipino students to rate them, just as they would on any personality test. Early studies using the same statistical techniques that have been used to test the FFM were used and demonstrated that seven, not five, dimensions were necessary to describe the Filipino personality adequately (Church, Katigbak, & Reyes,  1998 ; Church, Reyes, Katigbak, & Grimm,  1997 ). The two additional traits were Tempermentalness and Self-Assurance. In fact, similar types of findings were found previously with Spanish-speaking samples in Europe as well (Benet-Martinez & Waller,  1995  1997 ).

In one of their later studies, Church and colleagues (Katigbak, Church, Guanzon-Lapena, Carlota, & del Pilar,  2002 ) used two Filipino indigenous personality scales encompassing a total of 463 trait adjectives, and a Filipino version of the NEO PI-R to measure the FFM, and asked 511 college students in the Philippines to complete these measures. Statistical analyses indicated that there was considerable overlap in the personality dimensions that emerged from the Filipino scales and the FFM measured by the NEO PI-R. Still, several indigenous factors emerged, including Pagkamadaldal (Social Curiosity), Pagkamapagsapalaran (Risk-Taking), and Religiosity. These latter traits were especially important in predicting behaviors such as smoking, drinking, gambling, praying, tolerance of homosexuality, and tolerance of premarital and extramarital relations, above and beyond what could be predicted by the FFM.

Dominance

In the mid-20th century, European psychologists suggested the existence of an “authoritarian personality,” and developed scales to measure it (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, & Levinson,  1950 ). This dimension is related to the concept of dominance, and refers to the fact that people differ in their dependence on authority and hierarchical status differences among interactants. Hofstede, Bond, and Luk ( 1993 ) analyzed data from 1,300 individuals in Denmark and the Netherlands, and found six personality dimensions. Five of these were related to the FFM; the sixth, however, was not. The researchers labeled this “Authoritarianism.”

Actually, Dominance is a trait that emerges in studies of the personalities of animals. King and Figueredo ( 1997 ), for instance, presented 43 trait adjectives with representative items from the FFM to zoo trainers who work with chimpanzees in 12 zoos. The trainers were asked to describe the chimpanzees in terms of the adjectives provided. The results showed no differences between the zoos, and the interrater reliability among the raters was high.  Factor analysis  of the ratings produced six factors, five of which corresponded to the FFM; the sixth corresponded to dominance. The same findings have been reported in studies of orangutans and chimpanzees (Pederson, King, & Landau,  2005 ; Weiss, King, & Enns,  2002 ; Weiss, King, & Figueredo,  2000 ), and suggest that Dominance is an inherited trait among animals.

Summary

To date, attempts to find other universal traits do not contradict the FFM, but instead add to it. The unresolved question concerns exactly what other dimensions, if any, reliably exist across cultures. The findings reported above are indeed promising in terms of an answer to this question, but certainly much more research is necessary across a wider range of cultures to gauge its comparability with the FFM. Other indigenous approaches to studying traits have also been developed in countries such as India, Korea, Russia, and Greece (Allik et al.,  2009 ; Cheung, Cheung, Wada, & Zhang,  2003 ; Saucier, Georgiades, Tsaousis, & Goldberg,  2005 ). These, and other approaches, will hopefully shed more light on this important topic in the future.

To be sure, we need to be clear about the difference between the FFM, which is a model of the universal personality traits, and FFT, which is a theory about the source of those traits. It is entirely possible that the FFM will be amended in the future to allow for the possibility of other traits, but for the theory underlying them to be the same. Or it could be that the FFM will turn out to be the most reliable but that the theory accounting for the source is entirely wrong. The number of traits that are universal and where they come from are two issues we need to keep separate in our minds.

Internal versus External Locus of Control

Aside from cross-cultural research on traits, there has also been a considerable amount of cross-cultural research examining other dimensions of personality that do not fall cleanly within the trait perspective but are noteworthy in their own right. One of these concerns the personality concept of locus of control . This concept was developed by Rotter ( 1954  1966 ), who suggested that people differ in how much control they believe they have over their behavior and their relationship with their environment and with others. According to this schema, locus of control can be perceived as either internal or external to the individual. People with an internal locus of control see their behavior and relationships with others as dependent on their own behavior. Believing that your grades are mostly dependent on how much effort you put into study is an example of internal locus of control. People with an external locus of control see their behavior and relationships with the environment and others as contingent on forces outside themselves and beyond their control. If you believed your grades were mostly dependent on luck, the teacher’s benevolence, or the ease of the tests, you would be exemplifying an external locus of control.

Research examining locus of control has shown both similarities and differences across cultures. In general, European Americans have higher internal locus of control scores than East Asians, Swedes, Zambians, Zimbabweans, African Americans, Filipinos, and Brazilians (for example, Hamid,  1994 ; Lee and Dengerink,  1992 ; Munro,  1979 ; Dyal,  1984 ; Paguio, Robinson, Skeen, & Deal,  1987 ). These findings have often been interpreted as reflecting the mainstream American culture’s focus on individuality, separateness, and uniqueness, in contrast to a more balanced view of interdependence among individuals and between individuals and natural and supernatural forces found in many other cultures. People of non-mainstream American cultures may be more likely to see the causes of events and behaviors in sources that are external to themselves, such as fate, luck, supernatural forces, or relationships with others. Americans, however, prefer to take more personal responsibility for events and situations, and view themselves as having more personal control over such events.

Although such interpretations are interesting and provocative, they still leave some gaps to be filled. For example, they do not account for phenomena such as self-serving bias or defensive attributions, in which Americans tend to place the responsibility for negative events on others, not themselves (see  Chapter 13  on self-enhancement). Also, some researchers have suggested that locus of control is really a multifaceted construct spanning many different domains—academic achievement, work, interpersonal relationships, and so on—and that separate assessments of each of these domains are necessary to make meaningful comparisons on this construct. Smith, Dugan, and Trompenaars ( 1997 ) examined locus of control across 14 countries, and found some cross-national differences in locus of control, but larger differences by gender and status across countries. Thus, the search for cross-cultural differences may obscure larger differences based on other social constructs. Future research needs to address all these concerns to further elucidate the nature of cultural influences on locus of control.

Direct, Indirect, Proxy, and Collective Control

Yamaguchi ( 2001 ) has offered another interesting way of understanding control across cultures. He distinguishes between direct, indirect, proxy, and collective control. In  direct control , the self acts as an agent, and individuals feel themselves to be more self-efficacious when their agency is made explicit, leading to greater feelings of autonomy and efficacy. Direct control may be the preferred mode of behavior in cultural contexts that promote independence or autonomy, such as in the United States.

Other cultural contexts, however, may encourage other modes of control, primarily because of their focus on interpersonal harmony. For instance, in  indirect control , one’s agency is hidden or downplayed; people pretend as if they are not acting as an agent even though in reality they are doing so. Yamaguchi ( 2001 ) tells of an example in which a rakugo (comic master) was annoyed at his disciple’s loud singing. Instead of directly telling him to stop, he instead praised him with a loud voice. Although at first it sounded as if the comic master was praising the disciple, in reality he was telling him to be quiet; thus, the disciple stopped singing.

Proxy control  refers to control by someone else for the benefit of oneself. This is a form of control that can be used when personal control—either direct or indirect—is not available or inappropriate. These are third-party interventions, when intermediaries are called in to regulate or intervene in interpersonal relationships or conflicts between parties with potential or actual conflicts of interest. This type of control is essential for survival for those in weaker positions and thus unable to change their environments by themselves.

Finally, in  collective control , one attempts to control the environment as a member of a group, and the group serves as the agent of control. In this situation, individuals need to worry about interpersonal harmony less because the group shares the goal of control.

Yamaguchi ( 2001 ) suggests that direct, personal control may be the strategy of choice in cultures that value autonomy and independence, such as the United States. In cultures that value the maintenance of interpersonal harmony, however, indirect, proxy, and collective control strategies may be more prevalent ( Figure 10.3 ).

Figure 10.3 The Relationships Between Cultural Values and Preferred Control Strategies

 

Source: Yamaguchi, S. ( 2001 ). Culture and control orientations. In D. Matsumoto (Ed.), The handbook of culture and psychology (pp. 223-243). New York: Oxford University Press. ( www.oup.com ) By permission of Oxford University Press.

Autonomy

Deci and Ryan (Deci & Ryan,  1985 ; Ryan & Deci,  2000 ) have posited a self-determination theory,which states that people from all cultures share basic psychological needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness, but that the specific ways in which these needs are met and expressed differ according to context and culture. Meeting these needs, in whatever form or by whatever means, should be related to greater well-being of people in all cultures.

Of these claims, perhaps the most controversial is the one concerning autonomy. Conceptualizations of cultures that focus on individualism versus collectivism, and particularly those rooted in Markus and Kitayama’s ( 1991b ) framework of independent versus interdependent self-construals ( Chapter 13 ), suggests that people of collectivistic cultures are not autonomous. Deci and Ryan suggest, however, that there is a large distinction among autonomy, individualism, independence, and separateness. According to self-determination theory, people are autonomous when their behavior is experienced as willingly enacted and when they fully endorse the actions in which they are engaged or the values expressed by them (Chirkov, Ryan, Kim, & Kaplan,  2003 ). Thus, people are autonomous whenever they act in accord with their interests, values, or desires. The opposite of autonomy in this perspective is not dependence, but heteronomy, in which one’s actions are perceived as controlled by someone else or are otherwise alien to oneself. Thus, one can be either autonomously independent or dependent; they are separate constructs.

These ideas have received support in several studies involving participants from South Korea, Turkey, Russia, Canada, Brazil, and the United States (Chirkov et al.,  2003 ; Chirkov, Ryan, & Willness,  2005 ). In all cultures tested to date, their studies have shown that individuals tend to internalize different cultural practices, whatever those practices may be, and that despite those different practices, the relative autonomy of an individual’s motivations to engage in those practices predicts well-being. Autonomy, therefore, appears to be a universal psychological need and phenomenon, although the way in which it is practiced and expressed is different in different cultures (Kagitcibasi,  1996 ). This idea is bolstered by findings demonstrating the universality of self-efficacy—an optimistic sense of personal competence—a construct related to autonomy (Scholz, Hutierrez Dona, Sud, & Schwarzer,  2002 ).

INDIGENOUS PERSONALITIES AND A CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE ON IDENTITIES

As stated earlier in the chapter, indigenous personalities are conceptualizations of personality developed in a particular culture that are specific and relevant only to that culture. In general, not only are the concepts of personality rooted in and derived from the particular cultural group under question, but the methods used to test and examine those concepts are also particular to that culture. Thus, in contrast to much of the research described so far on universal traits, in which standardized personality measures are used to assess personality dimensions, studies of indigenous personalities often use their own nonstandardized methods.

Indigenous conceptions of personality are important because they give us a glimpse of how each culture believes it is important to carve up their psychological world. By identifying indigenous concepts, each culture pays tribute to a specific way of understanding their world, which is an important part of each cultural worldview. By giving these concepts names, each culture is then allowed to talk about them, thereby ensuring each indigenous concept’s special place in their culture.

Over the years, many scientists have been interested in indigenous conceptions of personality, and have described many different personality constructs considered to exist only in specific cultures. Early work in this area produced findings of many other personality constructs thought to be culture-specific, including the personality of Arabs (Beit-Hallahmi,  1972 ), North Alaskan Eskimos (Hippler,  1974 ), the Japanese (Sakamoto & Miura,  1976 ), the Fulam of Nigeria (Lott & Hart,  1977 ), the Irulas of Palamalai (Narayanan & Ganesan,  1978 ), Samoans (Holmes, Tallman, & Jantz,  1978 ), South African Indians (Heaven & Rajab,  1983 ), and the Ibo of Nigeria (Akin-Ogundeji,  1988 ). Berry, Poortinga, Segall, and Dasen ( 1992 ) examined three indigenous personality concepts, each of which was fundamentally different from American or Western concepts. The African model of personality, for example, views personality as consisting of three layers, each representing a different aspect of the person. The first layer, found at the core of the person and personality, embodies a spiritual principle; the second layer involves a psychological vitality principle; the third layer involves a physiological vitality principle. The body forms the outer framework that houses all these layers of the person. In addition, family lineage and community affect different core aspects of the African personality (Sow, 1977, 1978, cited in Berry et al.,  1992 ; see also Vontress, 1991 ).

Doi ( 1973 ) has postulated amae as a core concept of the Japanese personality. The root of this word means “sweet,” and loosely translated, amae refers to the passive, childlike dependence of one person on another, and is rooted in mother-child relationships. According to Doi, all Japanese relationships can be characterized by amae, which serves as a fundamental building block of Japanese culture and personality. This fundamental interrelationship between higher- and lower-status people in Japan serves as a major component not only of individual psychology but of interpersonal relationships, and it does so in ways that are difficult to grasp from a North American individualistic point of view.

Along with different conceptualizations of personality, different cultures have different, specific, important concepts that are important to understanding individuals in their culture. These include the Korean concept of cheong (human affection; Choi, Kim, & Choi,  1993 ); the Indian concept ofhishkama karma (detachment; Sinha,  1993 ); the Chinese concept ren qing (relationship orientation; Cheung, Leung, Fan, Song, Zhang, & Zhang,  1996 ); the Mexican concept simpatia (harmony, avoidance of conflict; Triandis, Marin, Lisansky, & Betancourt,  1984 ; Holloway, Waldrip, & Ickes, 2009 ); and the Filipino concepts of pagkikipagkapwa (shared identity), pakikiramdam (sensitivity, empathy), and pakikisama (going along with others; Ennquez, 1992) (all cited in Church,  2000 , p. 654).

Much of the work on indigenous personality has provided fuel for those who subscribe to the view that culture and personality are mutually constituted. In this view, it makes no sense to consider personality as a universal construct (like traits); instead, it makes more sense to understand each culture’s personalities as they exist and have developed within that culture. This viewpoint rejects the notion of a universal organization to personality that may have genetic, biological, and evolutionary components. Its proponents (Markus & Kitayama,  1998 ; Shweder & Bourne,  1984 ) argue that the research supporting universality and its possible biological substrates may be contaminated by the methods used. These methods, the argument goes, have been developed in American or European research laboratories by American or European researchers; because of this cultural bias, the findings support the FFM as a default by-product of the methods used to test it. Indigenous approaches, it is claimed, are immune from such bias because their methods are centered around concepts and practices that are local to the culture being studied (see, however, the replication of the FFM using nontraditional methods of assessing taxonomies of trait adjectives in multiple languages; De Raad, Perugini, Hrebickova, & Szarota,  1998 ).

INTEGRATING UNIVERSAL AND CULTURE-SPECIFIC UNDERSTANDING OF PERSONALITY

We believe there is a middle ground that integrates both universal and culturespecific understandings and empirical findings on personality. This middle ground starts with our understanding of personality as a multidimensional construct. If, as we have done at the beginning of this chapter, we broadly conceptualize two different aspects of personality, one involving traits and the other involving identities, then we can easily consider that they come from different sources and are influenced differently by biology and culture. On one hand, it appears that traits are more enduring aspects of a person’s personality, referring to underlying dispositions for thoughts, feelings, and actions. These appear to be at least somewhat rooted in biology and genetics; thus individuals are born with a set of genetic predispositions for certain aspects of their personalities. Because these are biologically-based genetic predispositions, they are relatively less impervious to cultural and environmental influences (although the exact degree of potential influence is an interesting question if one considers the possible influence of culture on biological processes across evolution).

On the other hand, identities, which is a loose term that refers to perceived roles in life, aggregate role and life experiences, narratives, values, motives, and the conceptualization and understanding of oneself, should be less influenced by biology and more influenced by culture because these are in large part cultural constructions of the meaning and value of one’s thoughts, feelings, and actions. As such, they are more likely to be “mutually constituted” in development, arising out of an interaction between the individual and the environment. During these interactions, culturally-determined meanings of right and wrong, good and bad, appropriate and inappropriate help to guide the construction of meaning, and thus the creation of identities, roles, and motives. It is no wonder, therefore, that this aspect of personality is less influenced by biology and more heavily influenced by culture.

This integrative perspective allows us to move beyond questioning whether personality is universal or culture-specific, as if they are mutually exclusive, dichotomous categories. A better and more fruitful approach might be to consider how some aspects are influenced relatively more by biology and how some other aspects are influenced relatively more by culture. It is entirely possible that some aspects of personality (e.g., traits) may be organized in a universal fashion, either because of biological or genetic factors or because of culture-constant learning and responses to the environment. The fact that some aspects of personality may be organized universally, however, does not necessarily argue against the possibility that other aspects of personality may be culturally unique. It may be these culturally unique aspects that give personality its own special flavor in each specific cultural milieu, and allow researchers the possibility of studying aspects of personality that they might not observe in other cultures. This is, in fact, the major premise underlying Five-Factor Theory that we discussed earlier. Thus, a more beneficial way of understanding the relationship between culture and personality may be to see indigenous and universal aspects of personality as two sides of the same coin, rather than as mutually exclusive. If we come to understand the relationship between culture and personality (and biology, for that matter) in ways that allow for the coexistence of universality and indigenization, then we can tackle the problem of exactly how to conceptualize and study this coexistence.

In terms of research findings, evidence for indigenous conceptions of personality are not necessarily antithetical to the existence of universal personality traits such as the FFM described earlier in this chapter. Both the FFM and indigenous personality concepts are theoretical constructs—they are inferences scientists make about the psychological underpinnings of a person’s personality. As we suggest, here the existence of one way of viewing personality does not necessarily argue against the existence of another. The two may exist simultaneously. Trait approaches such as the FFM refer more to the universal aspects of personality that are true of all people regardless of culture (underlying dispositional traits and action tendencies), while indigenous aspects of personality refer to those aspects of personality that are culture-specific, especially concerning their understandings and conceptualizations of personality. Both may be accurate.

Recent research that directly examines competing hypotheses from a universal trait perspective as opposed to a cultural, indigenous perspective of personality also sheds light on how both types of personalities exist and are differentially influenced by biology and culture. The universal trait view of personality suggests that traits exist in all cultures, and influence behavior in multiple contexts, because traits are inherent to people regardless of context. The indigenous view of personality, however, suggests that traits would not be endorsed or even existing in all cultures, and that even if they did, they would not influence behaviors across different contexts. Two studies, however, have shown that traits are endorsed even implicitly across cultures, and cross-context consistency in traits exist across cultures, and this consistency is related to adjustment similarly across cultures, demonstrating support for the universal trait view of personality (Church et al.,  2008 ; Church et al., 2006 ). At the same time, cultural differences in self-perceptions of traitedness existed, which supported indigenous, culture-specific perspectives. It makes sense that self-perceptions were more culturally variable, because these are more influenced by cultural meaning and construction. Perceptions of traits are different than the actual traits themselves.

The integrative perspective we suggest here proposes two separate but not mutually exclusive possibilities about the sources of personality: (1) the existence of biologically innate and evolutionarily adaptive factors that create genetic predispositions to certain types of personality traits and (2) the possibility of cultureconstant learning principles and processes (MacDonald,  1998 ; McCrae,  2000 ). Dispositional traits that humans bring with them into the world may be modified and adapted throughout development and the life span via interactions with the environment. Over time, dipping into this resource pool in order to adapt to various situational contexts may serve as the impetus for changes to the pool itself, which may account for changes in consistency and mean levels of the dispositional traits observed in previous studies (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000 ; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer,  2006 ).

CONCLUSION

In this chapter, we have discussed the major approaches to understanding and studying the relationship between culture and personality, and have examined many different types of studies on this topic. We began by defining personality and briefly describing major approaches to the topic. We described research on the FFM, which suggests that there is universality in personality organization around a small set of basic personality traits. Additional studies in this genre have suggested that there may be a sixth or even seventh personality trait that is universal; future research is necessary to test this idea more fully. We also discussed the FFT, a theory about where the universal personality traits come from. FFT suggests that the underlying traits reflect biologically based, inherited dispositions for behavior. But, how these traits are expressed may be culturally variable, as each person develops characteristic adaptations to address each of the traits.

In addition, we discussed interesting new cross-cultural research on control and autonomy. These studies are important because they inform us about personality organization from a different perspective. The evidence to date suggests that autonomy is a universal personality construct, and that all individuals of all cultures are autonomous. How we exert control over the environment in managing that autonomy, however, may differ in different contexts. That is, how we exert our personalities may be tactical.

Research on indigenous approaches to personality has demonstrated culturally specific aspects of personality that cannot be accounted for by the FFM. These two seemingly disparate sets of findings suggest a conflict in our understanding of the relationship between culture and personality. We presented above, however, an integrative theoretical perspective that suggests that these two seemingly opposing viewpoints need not be seen as mutually exclusive; rather, it may be more beneficial to view them as different, coexisting aspects of personality. The challenge for future research is to capture this coexistence, examining the relative degree of contribution of biological and cultural factors in the development and organization of personality. Future theories and studies will likely benefit from a blending of universal, etic approaches with indigenous, emic approaches (Cheung, van de Vijver, & Leong,  2011 , January 24).

EXPLORATION AND DISCOVERY

Why Does This Matter to Me?

· 1. Have you ever taken a personality test? Did you think the results were applicable to you or not? Why or why not? Do you think that such a test would be applicable in another cultural context?

· 2. What makes you unique? And how important is it to you to have that unique aspect of yourself? Do you think people of other cultures consider uniqueness and important aspect of themselves?

· 3. When there is an obstacle to your goal, how do you prefer to deal with it? Head on? Go around it? People of other cultures may prefer a different way of dealing with such obstacles. How will you manage when you have to work with others with vastly different perspectives on how to deal with problems?

Suggestions for Further Exploration

· 1. How predictive of actual behavior do you think personality tests are? How would you conduct a study that examines this question? How would you do it across cultures?

· 2. Do you believe animals have personalities? How would you study that and document those personalities?

· 3. Are indigenous personalities really indigenous? For example, the Japanese culture includes the concept of amae described above. Do you think that amae also exists in other cultures, at least in terms of behaviors or mental processes? How would you go about showing that?

GLOSSARY

collective control

A type of control in which one attempts to control the environment as a member of a group, and the group serves as the agent of control.

direct control

A type of control in which the self acts as an agent, and individuals feel themselves to be more self-efficacious when their agency is made explicit, leading to greater feelings of autonomy and efficacy. Direct control may be the preferred mode of behavior in cultural contexts that promote independence or autonomy, such as in the United States.

factor analysis (exploratory)

A statistical technique that allows researchers to group items on a questionnaire. The theoretical model underlying factor analysis is that groups of items on a questionnaire are answered in similar ways because they are assessing the same, single underlying psychological construct (or trait). By interpreting the groupings underlying the items, therefore, researchers make inferences about the underlying traits that are being measured.

identity

our perceived roles in life, aggregate role and life experiences, narratives, values, and motives.

indigenous personalities

Conceptualizations of personality developed in a particular culture that are specific and relevant only to that culture.

indirect control

A type of control in which one’s agency is hidden or downplayed; people pretend as if they are not acting as an agent even though they are doing so in reality.

locus of control

People’s attributions of control over their behaviors and relationships as internal or external to themselves. People with an internal locus of control see their behavior and relationships with others as dependent on their own behavior. People with an external locus of control see their behavior and relationships as contingent on forces outside themselves and beyond their control.

national character

The perception that each culture has a modal personality type, and that most persons in that culture share aspects of it.

personality

A set of relatively enduring behavioral and cognitive characteristics, traits, or predispositions that people take with them to different situations, contexts, and interactions with others, and that contribute to differences among individuals.

proxy control

Refers to control by someone else for the benefit of oneself. This is a form of control that can be used when personal control—either direct or indirect—is not available or inappropriate. These are third-party interventions.

trait

A characteristic or quality distinguishing a person. It refers to a consistent pattern of behavior that a person would usually display in relevant circumstances.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Mentalism And Radical Behaviorism homework help

Mentalism And Radical Behaviorism homework help

Mentalism and Radical Behaviorism

As a future ABA professional, understanding how the field of behavior analysis differs from other psychology fields is important. Behavior analysts use objective and behavioristic statements to describe phenomena they observe. At this point in your studies, it is important to start eliminating mentalistic statements from your vocabulary to help you to begin thinking like a behavior analyst.

Use the Mentalism and Radical Behaviorism Template provided in the Resources area of this assignment to complete the following:

  • For the 10 phrases or sentences provided in the template, identify whether the statement provides a mentalistic or a behavioristic explanation for a behavior.
  • Write a summary, explaining the reasoning behind your choices. You are not required to provide an individual explanation for each statement.
  • For each of the mentalistic explanations you identify, provide a behavioristic explanation as an alternative.
  • Analyze how the behavioristic approach is different from most other psychology fields.
    • How would a mentalistic approach inform one’s practice?
    • How would a behavior analytic approach inform one’s practice?

Assignment Requirements

  • Written communication: Should be free of errors that detract from the overall message.
  • APA formatting: References and citations are formatted according to current APA style guidelines.
  • Resources: Minimum of 1–2 scholarly or professional resources.
  • Length: 2–4 double-spaced pages, in addition to the title page and reference page.
  • Font and font size: Times New Roman, 12 point.

image2.png

image1.png

Mentalism and Radical Behaviorism

Phrases

Mentalistic or behavioristic

Explanation

If mentalisitic, provide an alternative

Bobby dropped his pencil and got out of his seat to retrieve it each time the teacher asked him to write his name.      
Ava was very happy at school today. Her teacher stated that she loved playing with other children at school.      
Stacy calls to make an appointment at her doctor’s office, because she knows she has the flu.      
Dana ate cake after having a long day to uplift her emotions.      
Every time the instructor asks Johnny to wash his hands, he walks to the sink, turns the water on, and washes his hands.      
Elizabeth follows her mother’s directions, because she knows she will be rewarded for appropriate behavior when she does.      
Kevin broke his pencil, because he was frustrated with the questions on his math test.      
Each time the buzzer sounds, the rat approaches the lever and presses it. When this occurs, a food pellet is produced.      
It is observed that each time the light is on, the rat presses the lever and a food pellet is delivered.      
Alicia got a 100% on her exam, because she is smart.      

Analyze how the behavioristic approach is different from most other psychology fields:

(be sure to include references.)

1

2

image1.png image2.png

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Exploring Popular Assumptions homework Assignment

Exploring Popular Assumptions homework Assignment

 

Beth Morling – Research Methods in Psychology_ Evaluating a World of Information.pdf

 

 

THIRD EDITION

Research Methods in Psychology EVALUATING A WORLD OF INFORMATION

 

 

 

THIRD EDITION

Research Methods in Psychology EVALUATING A WORLD OF INFORMATION

Beth Morling UNIVERSITY OF DELAWARE

n W. W. NORTON & COMPANY, INC.NEW YORK • LONDON

 

 

W. W. Norton & Company has been independent since its founding in 1923,

when William Warder Norton and Mary D. Herter Norton first published

lectures delivered at the People’s Institute, the adult education division of

New York City’s Cooper Union. The firm soon expanded its program beyond

the Institute, publishing books by celebrated academics from America and

abroad. By midcentury, the two major pillars of Norton’s publishing program—

trade books and college texts—were firmly established. In the 1950s, the Norton

family transferred control of the company to its employees, and today—with

a staff of four hundred and a comparable number of trade, college, and

professional titles published each year—W. W. Norton & Company stands as

the largest and oldest publishing house owned wholly by its employees.

Copyright © 2018, 2015, 2012 by W. W. Norton & Company, Inc.

All rights reserved Printed in Canada

Editor: Sheri L. Snavely Project Editor: David Bradley Editorial Assistant: Eve Sanoussi Manuscript/Development Editor: Betsy Dilernia Managing Editor, College: Marian Johnson Managing Editor, College Digital Media: Kim Yi Production Manager: Jane Searle Media Editor: Scott Sugarman Associate Media Editor: Victoria Reuter Media Assistant: Alex Trivilino Marketing Manager, Psychology: Ashley Sherwood Design Director and Text Design: Rubina Yeh Photo Editor: Travis Carr Photo Researcher: Dena Digilio Betz Permissions Manager: Megan Schindel Composition: CodeMantra Illustrations: Electragraphics Manufacturing: Transcontinental Printing

Permission to use copyrighted material is included in the Credits section beginning on page 603.

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Names: Morling, Beth, author. Title: Research methods in psychology : evaluating a world of information / Beth Morling, University of Delaware. Description: Third Edition. | New York : W. W. Norton & Company, [2017] | Revised edition of the author’s Research methods in psychology, [2015] | Includes bibliographical references and index. Identifiers: LCCN 2017030401 | ISBN 9780393617542 (pbk.) Subjects: LCSH: Psychology—Research—Methodology—Textbooks. | Psychology, Experimental—Textbooks. Classification: LCC BF76.5 .M667 2017 | DDC 150.72—dc23 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2017030401

Text-Only ISBN 978-0-393-63017-6

W.  W. Norton & Company, Inc., 500 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10110 wwnorton.com W.  W. Norton & Company Ltd., 15 Carlisle Street, London W1D 3BS

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0

 

 

For my parents

 

 

vii

Brief Contents

PART I Introduction to Scientific Reasoning CHAPTER 1 Psychology Is a Way of Thinking 5

CHAPTER 2 Sources of Information: Why Research Is Best and How to Find It 25

CHAPTER 3 Three Claims, Four Validities: Interrogation Tools for Consumers of Research 57

PART II Research Foundations for Any Claim CHAPTER 4 Ethical Guidelines for Psychology Research 89

CHAPTER 5 Identifying Good Measurement 117

PART III Tools for Evaluating Frequency Claims CHAPTER 6 Surveys and Observations: Describing What People Do 153

CHAPTER 7 Sampling: Estimating the Frequency of Behaviors and Beliefs 179

PART IV Tools for Evaluating Association Claims CHAPTER 8 Bivariate Correlational Research 203

CHAPTER 9 Multivariate Correlational Research 237

PART V Tools for Evaluating Causal Claims CHAPTER 10 Introduction to Simple Experiments 273

CHAPTER 11 More on Experiments: Confounding and Obscuring Variables 311

CHAPTER 12 Experiments with More Than One Independent Variable 351

PART VI Balancing Research Priorities CHAPTER 13 Quasi-Experiments and Small-N Designs 389

CHAPTER 14 Replication, Generalization, and the Real World 425

Statistics Review Descriptive Statistics 457

Statistics Review Inferential Statistics 479

Presenting Results APA-Style Reports and Conference Posters 505

Appendix A Random Numbers and How to Use Them 545

Appendix B Statistical Tables 551

 

 

viii

BETH MORLING is Professor of Psychology at the University of  Delaware. She attended Carleton College in Northfield, Minnesota, and received her Ph.D. from the University of Massachusetts at Amherst. Before coming to Delaware, she held positions at Union College (New York) and Muhlenberg College (Pennsylvania). In  addition to teaching research methods at Delaware almost every semester, she also teaches undergraduate cultural psychology, a seminar on the self- concept, and a graduate course in the teaching of psychology. Her research in the area of cultural psychology explores how cultural practices shape people’s motivations. Dr. Morling has been a Fulbright scholar in Kyoto, Japan, and was the Delaware State Professor of the Year (2014), an award from the Council for Advancement and Support of Education (CASE) and the Carnegie Foundation for the Advancement of Teaching.

About the Author

 

 

ix

Preface

Students in the psychology major plan to pursue a tremendous variety of careers— not just becoming psychology researchers. So they sometimes ask: Why do we need to study research methods when we want to be therapists, social workers, teachers, lawyers, or physicians? Indeed, many students anticipate that research methods will be “dry,” “boring,” and irrelevant to their future goals. This book was written with these very students in mind—students who are taking their first course in research methods (usually sophomores) and who plan to pursue a wide variety of careers. Most of the students who take the course will never become researchers themselves, but they can learn to systematically navigate the research information they will encounter in empirical journal articles as well as in online magazines, print sources, blogs, and tweets.

I used to tell students that by conducting their own research, they would be able to read and apply research later, in their chosen careers. But the literature on learning transfer leads me to believe that the skills involved in designing one’s own studies will not easily transfer to understanding and critically assessing studies done by others. If we want students to assess how well a study supports its claims, we have to teach them to assess research. That is the approach this book takes.

Students Can Develop Research Consumer Skills To be a systematic consumer of research, students need to know what to priori- tize when assessing a study. Sometimes random samples matter, and sometimes they do not. Sometimes we ask about random assignment and confounds, and sometimes we do not. Students benefit from having a set of systematic steps to help them prioritize their questioning when they interrogate quantitative infor- mation. To provide that, this book presents a framework of three claims and four validities, introduced in Chapter 3. One axis of the framework is the three kinds of claims researchers (as well as journalists, bloggers, and commentators) might make: frequency claims (some percentage of people do X), association claims (X is associated with Y), and causal claims (X changes Y). The second axis of

 

 

x PREfACE

the  framework is the four validities that are generally agreed upon by methodol- ogists: internal, external, construct, and statistical.

The three claims, four validities framework provides a scaffold that is rein- forced throughout. The book shows how almost every term, technique, and piece of information fits into the basic framework.

The framework also helps students set priorities when evaluating a study. Good quantitative reasoners prioritize different validity questions depending on the claim. For example, for a frequency claim, we should ask about measurement (construct validity) and sampling techniques (external validity), but not about ran- dom assignment or confounds, because the claim is not a causal one. For a causal claim, we prioritize internal validity and construct validity, but external validity is generally less important.

Through engagement with a consumer-focused research methods course, students become systematic interrogators. They start to ask more appropriate and refined questions about a study. By the end of the course, students can clearly explain why a causal claim needs an experiment to support it. They know how to evaluate whether a variable has been measured well. They know when it’s appro- priate to call for more participants in a study. And they can explain when a study must have a representative sample and when such a sample is not needed.

What About Future Researchers? This book can also be used to teach the flip side of the question: How can produc- ers of research design better studies? The producer angle is presented so that stu- dents will be prepared to design studies, collect data, and write papers in courses that prioritize these skills. Producer skills are crucial for students headed for Ph.D. study, and they are sometimes required by advanced coursework in the undergraduate major.

Such future researchers will find sophisticated content, presented in an accessible, consistent manner. They will learn the difference between media- tion (Chapter 9) and moderation (Chapters 8 and 9), an important skill in theory building and theory testing. They will learn how to design and interpret factorial designs, even up to three-way interactions (Chapter 12). And in the common event that a student-run study fails to work, one chapter helps them explore the possi- ble reasons for a null effect (Chapter 11). This book provides the basic statistical background, ethics coverage, and APA-style notes for guiding students through study design and execution.

Organization The fourteen chapters are arranged in six parts. Part I (Chapters 1–3) includes introductory chapters on the scientific method and the three claims, four validities framework. Part II (Chapters 4–5) covers issues that matter for any study: research

 

 

xiSupport for Students and Instructors

ethics and good measurement. Parts III–V (Chapters 6–12) correspond to each of the three claims (frequency, association, and causal). Part VI (Chapters 13–14) focuses on balancing research priorities.

Most of the chapters will be familiar to veteran instructors, including chapters on measurement, experimentation, and factorial designs. However, unlike some methods books, this one devotes two full chapters to correlational research (one on bivariate and one on multivariate studies), which help students learn how to interpret, apply, and interrogate different types of association claims, one of the common types of claims they will encounter.

There are three supplementary chapters, on Descriptive Statistics, Inferential Statistics, and APA-Style Reports and Conference Posters. These chapters provide a review for students who have already had statistics and provide the tools they need to create research reports and conference posters.

Two appendices—Random Numbers and How to Use Them, and Statistical Tables—provide reference tools for students who are conducting their own research.

Support for Students and Instructors The book’s pedagogical features emphasize active learning and repetition of the most important points. Each chapter begins with high-level learning objectives— major skills students should expect to remember even “a year from now.” Impor- tant terms in a chapter are introduced in boldface. The Check Your Understanding questions at the end of each major section provide basic questions that let students revisit key concepts as they read. Each chapter ends with multiple-choice Review Questions for retrieval practice, and a set of Learning Actively exercises that encourage students to apply what they learned. (Answers are provided at the end of the book.) A master table of the three claims and four validities appears inside the book’s front cover to remind students of the scaffold for the course.

I believe the book works pedagogically because it spirals through the three claims, four validities framework, building in repetition and depth. Although each chapter addresses the usual core content of research methods, students are always reminded of how a particular topic helps them interrogate the key validities. The interleaving of content should help students remember and apply this questioning strategy in the future.

I have worked with W. W. Norton to design a support package for fel- low instructors and students. The online Interactive Instructor’s Guide offers in-class activities, models of course design, homework and final assignments, and chapter-by-chapter teaching notes, all based on my experience with the course. The book is accompanied by other ancillaries to assist both new and experienced research methods instructors, including a new InQuizitive online assessment tool, a robust test bank with over 750 questions, updated lecture and active learning slides, and more; for a complete list, see p. xix.

 

 

xii PREfACE

Teachable Examples on the Everyday Research Methods Blog Students and instructors can find additional examples of psychological science in the news on my regularly updated blog, Everyday Research Methods (www .everydayresearchmethods.com; no password or registration required). Instruc- tors can use the blog for fresh examples to use in class, homework, or exams. Students can use the entries as extra practice in reading about research studies in psychology in the popular media. Follow me on Twitter to get the latest blog updates (@bmorling).

Changes in the Third Edition Users of the first and second editions will be happy to learn that the basic organi- zation, material, and descriptions in the text remain the same. The third edition provides several new studies and recent headlines. Inclusion of these new exam- ples means that instructors who assign the third edition can also use their favorite illustrations from past editions as extra examples while teaching.

In my own experience teaching the course, I found that students could often master concepts in isolation, but they struggled to bring them all together when reading a real study. Therefore, the third edition adds new Working It Through sections in several chapters (Chapters 3, 4, 5, 8, and 11). Each one works though a single study in depth, so students can observe how the chapter’s central concepts are integrated and applied. For instance, in Chapter 4, they can see how ethics concepts can be applied to a recent study that manipulated Facebook newsfeeds. The Working It Through material models the process students will probably use on longer class assignments.

Also new in the third edition, every figure has been redrawn to make it more visually appealing and readable. In addition, selected figures are annotated to help students learn how to interpret graphs and tables.

Finally, W. W. Norton’s InQuizitive online assessment tool is available with the third edition. InQuizitive helps students apply concepts from the textbook to practice examples, providing specific feedback on incorrect responses. Some questions require students to interpret tables and figures; others require them to apply what they’re learning to popular media articles.

Here is a detailed list of the changes made to each chapter.

 

 

xiiiChanges in the Third Edition

CHAPTER MAJOR CHANGES IN THE THIRD EDITION

1. Psychology Is a Way of Thinking

The heading structure is the same as in the second edition, with some updated examples. I replaced the facilitated communication example (still an excellent teaching example) with one on the Scared Straight program meant to keep adolescents out of the criminal justice system, based on a reviewer’s recommendation.

2. Sources of Information: Why Research Is Best and How to Find it

I simplified the coverage of biases of intuition. Whereas the second edition separated cognitive biases from motivated reasoning, the biases are now presented more simply. In addition, this edition aims to be clearer on the difference between the availability heuristic and the present/present bias. I also developed the coverage of Google Scholar.

3. Three Claims, Four Validities: Interrogation Tools for Consumers of Research

The three claims, four validities framework is the same, keeping the best teachable examples from the second edition and adding new examples from recent media. In response to my own students’ confusion, I attempted to clarify the difference between the type of study conducted (correlational or experimental) and the claims made about it. To this end, I introduced the metaphor of a gift, in which a journalist might “wrap” a correlational study in a fancy, but inappropriate, causal claim.

When introducing the three criteria for causation, I now emphasize that covariance is about the study’s results, while temporal precedence and internal validity are determined from the study’s method.

Chapter 3 includes the first new Working It Through section.

4. Ethical Guidelines for Psychology Research

I updated the section on animal research and removed the full text of APA Standard 8. There’s a new figure on the difference between plagiarism and paraphrasing, and a new example of research fabrication (the notorious, retracted Lancet article on vaccines and autism). A new Working It Through section helps students assess the ethics of a recent Facebook study that manipulated people’s newsfeeds.

5. Identifying Good Measurement

This chapter retains many of the teaching examples as the second edition. For clarity, I changed the discriminant validity example so the correlation is only weak (not both weak and negative). A new Working It Through section helps students apply the measurement concepts to a self-report measure of gratitude in relationships.

6. Surveys and Observations: Describing What People Do

Core examples are the same, with a new study illustrating the effect of leading questions (a poll on attitudes toward voter ID laws). Look for the new “babycam” example in the Learning Actively exercises.

7. Sampling: Estimating the Frequency of Behaviors and Beliefs

Look for new content on MTurk and other Internet-based survey panels. I updated the statistics on cell-phone-only populations, which change yearly. Finally, I added clarity on the difference between cluster and stratified samples and explained sample weighting.

I added the new keyword nonprobability sample to work in parallel with the term probability sample. A new table (Table 7.3) helps students group related terms.

 

 

xiv PREfACE

CHAPTER MAJOR CHANGES IN THE THIRD EDITION

8. Bivariate Correlational Research

This chapter keeps most of the second edition examples. It was revised to better show that association claims are separate from correlational methods. Look for improved moderator examples in this chapter. These new examples, I hope, will communicate to students that moderators change the relationship between variables; they do not necessarily reflect the level of one of the variables.

9. Multivariate Correlational Research

I replaced both of the main examples in this chapter. The new example of cross- lag panel design, on parental overpraise and child narcissism, has four time periods (rather than two), better representing contemporary longitudinal studies. In the multiple regression section, the recess example is replaced with one on adolescents in which watching sexual TV content predicts teen pregnancy. The present regression example is student-friendly and also has stronger effect sizes.

Look for an important change in Figure 9.13 aimed to convey that a moderator can be thought of as vulnerability. My own students tend to think something is a moderator when the subgroup is simply higher on one of the variables. For example, boys might watch more violent TV content and be higher on aggression, but that’s not the same as a moderator. Therefore, I have updated the moderator column with the moderator “parental discussion.” I hope this will help students come up with their own moderators more easily.

10. Introduction to Simple Experiments

The red/green ink example was replaced with a popular study on notetaking, comparing the effects of taking notes in longhand or on laptops. There is also a new example of pretest/posttest designs (a study on mindfulness training). Students sometimes are surprised when a real-world study has multiple dependent variables, so I’ve highlighted that more in the third edition. Both of the chapter’s opening examples have multiple dependent variables.

I kept the example on pasta bowl serving size. However, after Chapter 10 was typeset, some researchers noticed multiple statistical inconsistencies in several publications from Wansink’s lab (for one summary of the issues, see the Chronicle of Higher Education article, “Spoiled Science”). At the time of writing, the pasta study featured in Chapter 10 has not been identified as problematic. Nevertheless, instructors might wish to engage students in a discussion of these issues.

11. More on Experiments: Confounding and Obscuring Variables

The content is virtually the same, with the addition of two Working It Through sections. The first one is to show students how to work through Table 11.1 using the mindfulness study from Chapter 10. This is important because after seeing Table 11.1, students sometimes think their job is to find the flaw in any study. In fact, most published studies do not have major internal validity flaws. The second Working It Through shows students how to analyze a null result.

12. Experiments with More Than One Independent Variable

Recent work has suggested that context-specific memory effects are not robust, so I replaced the Godden and Baddeley factorial example on context-specific learning with one comparing the memory of child chess experts to adults.

 

 

xv

CHAPTER MAJOR CHANGES IN THE THIRD EDITION

13. Quasi-Experiments and Small-N Designs

I replaced the Head Start study for two reasons. First, I realized it’s not a good example of a nonequivalent control group posttest-only design, because it actually included a pretest! Second, the regression to the mean effect it meant to illustrate is rare and difficult to understand. In exchange, there is a new study on the effects of walking by a church.

In the small-N design section, I provided fresh examples of multiple baseline design and alternating treatment designs. I also replaced the former case study example (split-brain studies) with the story of H.M. Not only is H.M.’s story compelling (especially as told through the eyes of his friend and researcher Suzanne Corkin), the brain anatomy required to understand this example is also simpler than that of split- brain studies, making it more teachable.

14. Replication, Generalization, and the Real World

A significant new section and table present the so-called “replication crisis” in psychology. In my experience, students are extremely engaged in learning about these issues. There’s a new example of a field experiment, a study on the effect of radio programs on reconciliation in Rwanda.

Supplementary Chapters In the supplementary chapter on inferential statistics, I replaced the section on randomization tests with a new section on confidence intervals. The next edition of the book may transition away from null hypothesis significance testing to emphasize the “New Statistics” of estimation and confidence intervals. I welcome feedback from instructors on this potential change.

Changes in the Third Edition

 

 

xvi

Acknowledgments

Working on this textbook has been rewarding and enriching, thanks to the many people who have smoothed the way. To start, I feel fortunate to have collaborated with an author-focused company and an all-around great editor, Sheri Snavely. Through all three editions, she has been both optimistic and realistic, as well as savvy and smart. She also made sure I got the most thoughtful reviews possible and that I was supported by an excellent staff at Norton: David Bradley, Jane Searle, Rubina Yeh, Eve Sanoussi, Victoria Reuter, Alex Trivilino, Travis Carr, and Dena Diglio Betz. My developmental editor, Betsy Dilernia, found even more to refine in the third edition, making the language, as well as each term, figure, and refer- ence, clear and accurate.

I am also thankful for the support and continued enthusiasm I have received from the Norton sales management team: Michael Wright, Allen Clawson, Ashley Sherwood, Annie Stewart, Dennis Fernandes, Dennis Adams, Katie Incorvia, Jordan Mendez, Amber Watkins, Shane Brisson, and Dan Horton. I also wish to thank the science and media special- ists for their creativity and drive to ensure my book reaches a wide audience, and that all the media work for instructors and students.

I deeply appreciate the support of many col- leagues. My former student Patrick Ewell, now at Kenyon College, served as a sounding board for new examples and authored the content for InQuizitive. Eddie Brummelman and Stefanie Nelemans provided additional correlations for the cross-lag panel design in Chapter 9. My friend Carrie Smith authored the Test Bank for the past two editions and has made it

an authentic measure of quantitative reasoning (as well as sending me things to blog about). Catherine Burrows carefully checked and revised the Test Bank for the third edition. Many thanks to Sarah Ainsworth, Reid Griggs, Aubrey McCarthy, Emma McGorray, and Michele M. Miller for carefully and patiently fact-checking every word in this edition. My student Xiaxin Zhong added DOIs to all the refer- ences and provided page numbers for the Check Your Understanding answers. Thanks, as well, to Emily Stanley and Jeong Min Lee, for writing and revising the questions that appear in the Coursepack created for the course management systems. I’m grateful to Amy Corbett and Kacy Pula for reviewing the ques- tions in InQuizitive. Thanks to my students Matt Davila-Johnson and Jeong Min Lee for posing for photographs in Chapters 5 and 10.

The book’s content was reviewed by a cadre of talented research method professors, and I am grateful to each of them. Some were asked to review; others cared enough to send me comments or examples by e-mail. Their students are lucky to have them in the classroom, and my readers will benefit from the time they spent in improving this book:

Eileen Josiah Achorn, University of Texas, San Antonio Sarah Ainsworth, University of North Florida Kristen Weede Alexander, California State University,

Sacramento Leola Alfonso-Reese, San Diego State University Cheryl Armstrong, Fitchburg State University Jennifer Asmuth, Susquehanna University Kristin August, Rutgers University, Camden

 

 

xviiAcknowledgments

Jessica L. Barnack-Tavlaris, The College of New Jersey Gordon Bear, Ramapo College Margaret Elizabeth Beier, Rice University Jeffrey Berman, University of Memphis Brett Beston, McMaster University Alisa Beyer, Northern Arizona University Julie Boland, University of Michigan Marina A. Bornovalova, University of South Florida Caitlin Brez, Indiana State University Shira Brill, California State University, Northridge J. Corey Butler, Southwest Minnesota State University Ricardo R. Castillo, Santa Ana College Alexandra F. Corning, University of Notre Dame Kelly A. Cotter, California State University, Stanislaus Lisa Cravens-Brown, The Ohio State University Victoria Cross, University of California, Davis Matthew Deegan, University of Delaware Kenneth DeMarree, University at Buffalo Jessica Dennis, California State University, Los Angeles Nicole DeRosa, SUNY Upstate Golisano Children’s Hospital Rachel Dinero, Cazenovia College Dana S. Dunn, Moravian College C. Emily Durbin, Michigan State University Russell K. Espinoza, California State University, Fullerton Patrick Ewell, Kenyon College Iris Firstenberg, University of California, Los Angeles Christina Frederick, Sierra Nevada College Alyson Froehlich, University of Utah Christopher J. Gade, University of California, Berkeley Timothy E. Goldsmith, University of New Mexico Jennifer Gosselin, Sacred Heart University AnaMarie Connolly Guichard, California State University,

Stanislaus Andreana Haley, University of Texas, Austin Edward Hansen, Florida State University Cheryl Harasymchuk, Carleton University Richard A. Hullinger, Indiana State University Deborah L. Hume, University of Missouri Kurt R. Illig, University of St. Thomas Jonathan W. Ivy, Pennsylvania State University, Harrisburg W. Jake Jacobs, University of Arizona Matthew D. Johnson, Binghamton University Christian Jordan, Wilfrid Laurier University Linda Juang, San Francisco State University

Victoria A. Kazmerski, Penn State Erie, The Behrend College Heejung Kim, University of California, Santa Barbara Greg M. Kim-Ju, California State University, Sacramento Ari Kirshenbaum, Ph.D., St. Michael’s College Kerry S. Kleyman, Metropolitan State University Penny L. Koontz, Marshall University Christina M. Leclerc, Ph.D., State University of New York

at Oswego Ellen W. Leen-Feldner, University of Arkansas Carl Lejuez, University of Maryland Marianne Lloyd, Seton Hall University Stella G. Lopez, University of Texas, San Antonio Greg Edward Loviscky, Pennsylvania State University Sara J. Margolin, Ph.D., The College at Brockport, State

University of New York Azucena Mayberry, Texas State University Christopher Mazurek, Columbia College Peter Mende-Siedlecki, University of Delaware Molly A. Metz, Miami University Dr. Michele M. Miller, University of Illinois Springfield Daniel C. Molden, Northwestern University J. Toby Mordkoff, University of Iowa Elizabeth Morgan, Springfield College Katie Mosack, University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee Erin Quinlivan Murdoch, George Mason University Stephanie C. Payne, Texas A&M University Anita Pedersen, California State University, Stanislaus Elizabeth D. Peloso, University of Pennsylvania M. Christine Porter, College of William and Mary Joshua Rabinowitz, University of Michigan Elizabeth Riina, Queens College, City University of New York James R. Roney, University of California, Santa Barbara Richard S. Rosenberg, Ph.D., California State University,

Long Beach Carin Rubenstein, Pima Community College Silvia J. Santos, California State University, Dominguez Hills Pamela Schuetze, Ph.D., The College at Buffalo, State

University of New York John N. Schwoebel, Ph.D., Utica College Mark J. Sciutto, Muhlenberg College Elizabeth A. Sheehan, Georgia State University Victoria A. Shivy, Virginia Commonwealth University Leo Standing, Bishop’s University

 

 

xviii ACkNOwLEDGMENTs

Harold W. K. Stanislaw, California State University, Stanislaus Kenneth M. Steele, Appalachian State University Mark A. Stellmack, University of Minnesota, Twin Cities Eva Szeli, Arizona State University Lauren A. Taglialatela, Kennesaw State University Alison Thomas-Cottingham, Rider University Chantal Poister Tusher, Georgia State University Allison A. Vaughn, San Diego State University Simine Vazire, University of California, Davis Jan Visser, University of Groningen John L. Wallace, Ph.D., Ball State University Shawn L. Ward, Le Moyne College Christopher Warren, California State University, Long Beach Shannon N. Whitten, University of Central Florida Jelte M. Wicherts, Tilburg University Antoinette R. Wilson, University of California, Santa Cruz James Worthley, University of Massachusetts, Lowell Charles E. (Ted) Wright, University of California, Irvine Guangying Wu, The George Washington University

David Zehr, Plymouth State University Peggy Mycek Zoccola, Ohio University

I have tried to make the best possible improvements from all of these capable reviewers.

My life as a teaching professor has been enriched during the last few years because of the friendship and support of my students and colleagues at the Uni- versity of Delaware, colleagues I see each year at the SPSP conference, and all the faculty I see regularly at the National Institute for the Teaching of Psychology, affectionately known as NITOP.

Three teenage boys will keep a person both enter- tained and humbled; thanks to Max, Alek, and Hugo for providing their services. I remain grateful to my mother-in-law, Janet Pochan, for cheerfully helping on the home front. Finally, I want to thank my husband Darrin for encouraging me and for always having the right wine to celebrate (even if it’s only Tuesday).

Beth Morling

 

 

Media Resources for Instructors and Students

G

N

G

INTERACTIVE INsTRUCTOR’s GUIDE Beth Morling, University of Delaware The Interactive Instructor’s Guide contains hundreds of downloadable resources and teaching ideas, such as a discussion of how to design a course that best utilizes the textbook, sample syllabus and assignments, and chapter-by-chapter teaching notes and suggested activities.

POwERPOINTs The third edition features three types of PowerPoints. The Lecture PowerPoints provide an overview of the major headings and definitions for each chapter. The Art Slides contain a complete set of images. And the Active Learning Slides provide the author’s favorite in-class activities, as well as reading quiz- zes and clicker questions. Instructors can browse the Active Learning Slides to select activities that supplement their classes.

TEsT BANk C. Veronica Smith, University of Mississippi, and Catherine Burrows, University of Miami The Test Bank provides over 750 questions using an evidence-centered approach designed in collabora- tion with Valerie Shute of Florida State University and Diego Zapata-Rivera of the Educational Testing Service. The Test Bank contains multiple-choice and short-answer questions classified by section, Bloom’s taxonomy, and difficulty, making it easy for instructors to construct tests and quizzes that are meaningful and diagnostic. The Test Bank is available in Word RTF, PDF, and ExamView® Assessment Suite formats.

INQUIZITIVE Patrick Ewell, Kenyon College InQuizitive allows students to practice applying terminology in the textbook to numerous examples. It can guide the students with specific feedback for incorrect answers to help clarify common mistakes. This online assessment tool gives students the repetition they need to fully understand the material without cutting into valuable class time. InQuizitive provides practice in reading tables and figures, as well as identifying the research methods used in studies from popular media articles, for an integrated learning experience.

EVERYDAY REsEARCH METHODs BLOG: www.everydayresearchmethods.com The Research Methods in Psychology blog offers more than 150 teachable moments from the web, curated by Beth Morling and occasional guest contributors. Twice a month, the author highlights examples of psychological science in the news. Students can connect these recent stories with textbook concepts. Instructors can use blog posts as examples in lecture or assign them as homework. All entries are searchable by chapter.

COURsEPACk Emily Stanley, University of Mary Washington, and Jeong Min Lee, University of Delaware The Coursepack presents students with review opportunities that employ the text’s analytical frame- work. Each chapter includes quizzes based on the Norton Assessment Guidelines, Chapter Outlines created by the textbook author and based on the Learning Objectives in the text, and review flash- cards. The APA-style guidelines from the textbook are also available in the Coursepack for easy access.

H

r

C

xix

 

 

xx

Contents

Preface ix Media Resources for Instructors and Students xix

PART I Introduction to Scientific Reasoning

CHAPTER 1

Psychology Is a Way of Thinking 5

Research Producers, Research Consumers 6 Why the Producer Role Is Important 6

Why the Consumer Role Is Important 7

The Benefits of Being a Good Consumer 8

How Scientists Approach Their Work 10 Scientists Are Empiricists 10

Scientists Test Theories: The Theory-Data Cycle 11

Scientists Tackle Applied and Basic Problems 16

Scientists Dig Deeper 16

Scientists Make It Public: The Publication Process 17

Scientists Talk to the World: From Journal to

Journalism 17

Chapter Review 22

Contents

 

 

xxiContents

CHAPTER 2

Sources of Information: Why Research Is Best and How to Find It 25

The Research vs. Your Experience 26 Experience Has No Comparison Group 26

Experience Is Confounded 29

Research Is Better Than Experience 29

Research Is Probabilistic 31

The Research vs. Your Intuition 32 Ways That Intuition Is Biased 32

The Intuitive Thinker vs. the Scientific Reasoner 38

Trusting Authorities on the Subject 39 Finding and Reading the Research 42 Consulting Scientific Sources 42

Finding Scientific Sources 44

Reading the Research 46

Finding Research in Less Scholarly Places 48

Chapter Review 53

CHAPTER 3

Three Claims, Four Validities: Interrogation Tools for Consumers of Research 57

Variables 58 Measured and Manipulated Variables 58

From Conceptual Variable to Operational Definition 59

Three Claims 61 Frequency Claims 62

Association Claims 63

Causal Claims 66

Not All Claims Are Based on Research 68

Interrogating the Three Claims Using the Four Big Validities 68 Interrogating Frequency Claims 69

Interrogating Association Claims 71

Interrogating Causal Claims 74

Prioritizing Validities 79

Review: Four Validities, Four Aspects of Quality 80 wORkING IT THROUGH Does Hearing About Scientists’ Struggles Inspire

Young Students? 81

Chapter Review 83

 

 

xxii CONTENTs

PART II Research Foundations for Any Claim

CHAPTER 4

Ethical Guidelines for Psychology Research 89

Historical Examples 89 The Tuskegee Syphilis Study Illustrates Three Major Ethics Violations 89

The Milgram Obedience Studies Illustrate a Difficult Ethical Balance 92

Core Ethical Principles 94 The Belmont Report: Principles and Applications 94

Guidelines for Psychologists: The APA Ethical Principles 98 Belmont Plus Two: APA’s Five General Principles 98

Ethical Standards for Research 99

Ethical Decision Making: A Thoughtful Balance 110 wORkING IT THROUGH Did a Study Conducted on Facebook Violate Ethical

Principles? 111

Chapter Review 113

CHAPTER 5

Identifying Good Measurement 117

Ways to Measure Variables 118 More About Conceptual and Operational Variables 118

Three Common Types of Measures 120

Scales of Measurement 122

Reliability of Measurement: Are the Scores Consistent? 124 Introducing Three Types of Reliability 125

Using a Scatterplot to Quantify Reliability 126

Using the Correlation Coefficient r to Quantify Reliability 128

Reading About Reliability in Journal Articles 131

Validity of Measurement: Does It Measure What It’s Supposed to Measure? 132

Measurement Validity of Abstract Constructs 133

Face Validity and Content Validity: Does It Look Like a

Good Measure? 134

Criterion Validity: Does It Correlate with Key Behaviors? 135

Convergent Validity and Discriminant Validity: Does the

Pattern Make Sense? 139

The Relationship Between Reliability and Validity 142

 

 

xxiiiContents

Review: Interpreting Construct Validity Evidence 143

wORkING IT THROUGH How Well Can We Measure the Amount of Gratitude Couples Express to Each Other? 145

Chapter Review 147

PART III Tools for Evaluating Frequency Claims

CHAPTER 6

Surveys and Observations: Describing What People Do 153

Construct Validity of Surveys and Polls 153 Choosing Question Formats 154

Writing Well-Worded Questions 155

Encouraging Accurate Responses 159

Construct Validity of Behavioral Observations 165 Some Claims Based on Observational Data 165

Making Reliable and Valid Observations 169

Chapter Review 175

CHAPTER 7

Sampling: Estimating the Frequency of Behaviors and Beliefs 179

Generalizability: Does the Sample Represent the Population? 179 Populations and Samples 180

When Is a Sample Biased? 182

Obtaining a Representative Sample: Probability Sampling Techniques 186

Settling for an Unrepresentative Sample: Nonprobability Sampling Techniques 191

Interrogating External Validity: What Matters Most? 193 In a Frequency Claim, External Validity Is a

Priority 193

When External Validity Is a Lower Priority 194

Larger Samples Are Not More Representative 196

Chapter Review 198

 

 

xxiv CONTENTs

PART IV Tools for Evaluating Association Claims

CHAPTER 8

Bivariate Correlational Research 203

Introducing Bivariate Correlations 204 Review: Describing Associations Between Two Quantitative

Variables 205

Describing Associations with Categorical Data 207

A Study with All Measured Variables Is Correlational 209

Interrogating Association Claims 210 Construct Validity: How Well Was Each Variable Measured? 210

Statistical Validity: How Well Do the Data Support

the Conclusion? 211

Internal Validity: Can We Make a Causal Inference from

an Association? 221

External Validity: To Whom Can the Association Be Generalized? 226

wORkING IT THROUGH Are Parents Happier Than People with No Children? 231

Chapter Review 233

CHAPTER 9

Multivariate Correlational Research 237

Reviewing the Three Causal Criteria 238 Establishing Temporal Precedence with Longitudinal

Designs 239 Interpreting Results from Longitudinal Designs 239

Longitudinal Studies and the Three Criteria for Causation 242

Why Not Just Do an Experiment? 242

Ruling Out Third Variables with Multiple-Regression Analyses 244 Measuring More Than Two Variables 244

Regression Results Indicate If a Third Variable Affects

the Relationship 247

Adding More Predictors to a Regression 251

Regression in Popular Media Articles 252

Regression Does Not Establish Causation 254

Getting at Causality with Pattern and Parsimony 256 The Power of Pattern and Parsimony 256

Pattern, Parsimony, and the Popular Media 258

 

 

xxvContents

Mediation 259 Mediators vs. Third Variables 261

Mediators vs. Moderators 262

Multivariate Designs and the Four Validities 264 Chapter Review 266

PART V Tools for Evaluating Causal Claims

CHAPTER 10

Introduction to Simple Experiments 273

Two Examples of Simple Experiments 273 Example 1: Taking Notes 274

Example 2: Eating Pasta 275

Experimental Variables 276 Independent and Dependent Variables 277

Control Variables 278

Why Experiments Support Causal Claims 278 Experiments Establish Covariance 279

Experiments Establish Temporal Precedence 280

Well-Designed Experiments Establish Internal Validity 281

Independent-Groups Designs 287 Independent-Groups vs. Within-Groups Designs 287

Posttest-Only Design 287

Pretest/Posttest Design 288

Which Design Is Better? 289

Within-Groups Designs 290 Repeated-Measures Design 290

Concurrent-Measures Design 291

Advantages of Within-Groups Designs 292

Covariance, Temporal Precedence, and Internal Validity in Within-Groups Designs 294

Disadvantages of Within-Groups Designs 296

Is Pretest/Posttest a Repeated-Measures Design? 297

Interrogating Causal Claims with the Four Validities 298 Construct Validity: How Well Were the Variables Measured and Manipulated? 298

External Validity: To Whom or What Can the Causal Claim Generalize? 301

Statistical Validity: How Well Do the Data Support the Causal Claim? 304

Internal Validity: Are There Alternative Explanations for the Results? 306

Chapter Review 307

 

 

xxvi CONTENTs

CHAPTER 11

More on Experiments: Confounding and Obscuring Variables 311

Threats to Internal Validity: Did the Independent Variable Really Cause the Difference? 312

The Really Bad Experiment (A Cautionary Tale) 312

Six Potential Internal Validity Threats in One-Group,

Pretest/Posttest Designs 314

Three Potential Internal Validity Threats in Any Study 322

With So Many Threats, Are Experiments Still Useful? 325

wORkING IT THROUGH Did Mindfulness Training Really Cause GRE Scores to Improve? 328

Interrogating Null Effects: What If the Independent Variable Does Not Make a Difference? 330

Perhaps There Is Not Enough Between-Groups Difference 332

Perhaps Within-Groups Variability Obscured the Group Differences 335

Sometimes There Really Is No Effect to Find 342

wORkING IT THROUGH Will People Get More Involved in Local Government If They Know They’ll Be Publicly Honored? 344

Null Effects May Be Published Less Often 345

Chapter Review 346

CHAPTER 12

Experiments with More Than One Independent Variable 351

Review: Experiments with One Independent Variable 351 Experiments with Two Independent Variables Can

Show Interactions 353

Intuitive Interactions 353

Factorial Designs Study Two Independent Variables 355

Factorial Designs Can Test Limits 356

Factorial Designs Can Test Theories 358

Interpreting Factorial Results: Main Effects and Interactions 360

Factorial Variations 370 Independent-Groups Factorial Designs 370

Within-Groups Factorial Designs 370

Mixed Factorial Designs 371

Increasing the Number of Levels of an Independent Variable 371

Increasing the Number of Independent Variables 373

Identifying Factorial Designs in Your Reading 378 Identifying Factorial Designs in Empirical Journal Articles 379

Identifying Factorial Designs in Popular Media Articles 379

Chapter Review 383

 

 

xxviiContents

PART VI Balancing Research Priorities

CHAPTER 13

Quasi-Experiments and Small-N Designs 389

Quasi-Experiments 389 Two Examples of Independent-Groups

Quasi-Experiments 390

Two Examples of Repeated-Measures

Quasi-Experiments 392

Internal Validity in Quasi-Experiments 396

Balancing Priorities in Quasi-Experiments 404

Are Quasi-Experiments the Same as Correlational Studies? 405

Small-N Designs: Studying Only a Few Individuals 406 Research on Human Memory 407

Disadvantages of Small-N Studies 410

Behavior-Change Studies in Applied Settings:

Three Small-N Designs 411

Other Examples of Small-N Studies 417

Evaluating the Four Validities in Small-N Designs 418

Chapter Review 420

CHAPTER 14

Replication, Generalization, and the Real World 425

To Be Important, a Study Must Be Replicated 425 Replication Studies 426

The Replication Debate in Psychology 430

Meta-Analysis: What Does the Literature Say? 433

Replicability, Importance, and Popular Media 436

To Be Important, Must a Study Have External Validity? 438 Generalizing to Other Participants 438

Generalizing to Other Settings 439

Does a Study Have to Be Generalizable to Many People? 440

Does a Study Have to Take Place in a Real-World Setting? 447

Chapter Review 453

 

 

xxviii CONTENTs

Statistics Review Descriptive Statistics 457 Statistics Review Inferential Statistics 479 Presenting Results APA-Style Reports and Conference Posters 505 Appendix A Random Numbers and How to Use Them 545 Appendix B Statistical Tables 551 Areas Under the Normal Curve (Distribution of z) 551

Critical Values of t 557

Critical Values of F 559

r to z’ Conversion 564

Critical Values of r 565 Glossary 567 Answers to End-of-Chapter Questions 577 Review Question 577

Guidelines for Selected Learning Actively Exercises 578 References 589 Credits 603 Name Index 607 Subject Index 611

 

 

THIRD EDITION

Research Methods in Psychology EVALUATING A WORLD OF INFORMATION

 

 

 

PART I

Introduction to Scientific Reasoning

 

 

Your Dog Hates Hugs NYMag.com, 2016

Mindfulness May Improve Test Scores Scientific American, 2013

 

 

5

Psychology Is a Way of Thinking THINKING BACK TO YOUR introductory psychology course, what do you remember learning? You might remember that dogs can be trained to salivate at the sound of a bell or that people in a group fail to call for help when the room fills up with smoke. Or perhaps you recall studies in which people administered increasingly stron- ger electric shocks to an innocent man although he seemed to be in distress. You may have learned what your brain does while you sleep or that you can’t always trust your memories. But how come you didn’t learn that “we use only 10% of our brain” or that “hitting a punching bag can make your anger go away”?

The reason you learned some principles, and not others, is because psychological science is based on studies—on research—by psychologists. Like other scientists, psychologists are empiricists. Being an empiricist means basing one’s conclusions on systematic observations. Psychologists do not simply think intuitively about behavior, cognition, and emotion; they know what they know because they have conducted studies on people and animals acting in their natural environments or in specially designed situations. Research is what tells us that most people will administer electric shock to an innocent man in certain situations, and it also tells us that people’s brains are usually fully engaged—not just 10%. If you are to think like a psychologist, then you must think like a researcher, and taking a course in research methods is crucial to your understanding of psychology.

This book explains the types of studies psychologists conduct, as well as the potential strengths and limitations of each type of study. You will learn not only how to plan your own studies but

1 LEARNING OBJECTIVES

A year from now, you should still be able to:

1. Explain what it means to reason empirically.

2. Appreciate how psychological research methods help you become a better producer of information as well as a better consumer of information.

3. Describe five practices that psychological scientists engage in.

 

 

6 CHAPTER 1 Psychology Is a Way of Thinking

also how to find research, read about it, and ask questions about it. While gaining a greater appreciation for the rigorous standards psychologists maintain in their research, you’ll find out how to be a systematic and critical consumer of psychological science.

RESEARCH PRODUCERS, RESEARCH CONSUMERS Some psychology students are fascinated by the research process and intend to become producers of research. Perhaps they hope to get a job studying brain anatomy, documenting the behavior of dolphins or monkeys, administering per- sonality questionnaires, observing children in a school setting, or analyzing data. They may want to write up their results and present them at research meetings. These students may dream about working as research scientists or professors.

Other psychology students may not want to work in a lab, but they do enjoy reading about the structure of the brain, the behavior of dolphins or monkeys, the personalities of their fellow students, or the behavior of children in a school setting. They are interested in being consumers of research information—reading about research so they can later apply it to their work, hobbies, relationships, or personal growth. These students might pursue careers as family therapists, teachers, entrepreneurs, guidance counselors, or police officers, and they expect psychology courses to help them in these roles.

In practice, many psychologists engage in both roles. When they are planning their research and creating new knowledge, they study the work of others who have gone before them. Furthermore, psychologists in both roles require a curi- osity about behavior, emotion, and cognition. Research producers and consumers also share a commitment to the practice of empiricism—to answer psychological questions with direct, formal observations, and to communicate with others about what they have learned.

Why the Producer Role Is Important For your future coursework in psychology, it is important to know how to be a producer of research. Of course, students who decide to go to graduate school for psychology will need to know all about research methods. But even if you do not plan to do graduate work in psychology, you will probably have to write a paper following the style guidelines of the American Psychological Association (APA) before you graduate, and you may be required to do research as part of a course lab section. To succeed, you will need to know how to randomly assign people to groups, how to measure attitudes accurately, or how to interpret results from a graph. The skills you acquire by conducting research can teach you how psycho- logical scientists ask questions and how they think about their discipline.

 

 

7Research Producers, Research Consumers

As part of your psychology studies, you might even work in a research lab as an undergraduate (Figure 1.1). Many psy- chology professors are active researchers, and if you are offered the opportunity to get involved in their laboratories, take it! Your faculty supervisor may ask you to code behaviors, assign participants to different groups, graph an outcome, or write a report. Doing so will give you your first taste of being a research producer. Although you will be supervised closely, you will be expected to know the basics of conducting research. This book will help you understand why you have to protect the anonymity of your participants, use a cod- ing book, or flip a coin to decide who goes in which group. By participating as a research producer, you can expect to deepen your understanding of psychological inquiry.

Why the Consumer Role Is Important Although it is important to understand the psychologist’s role as a producer of research, most psychology majors do not eventually become researchers. Regard- less of the career you choose, however, becoming a savvy consumer of informa- tion is essential. In your psychology courses, you will read studies published by psychologists in scientific journals. You will need to develop the ability to read about research with curiosity—to understand it, learn from it, and ask appropriate questions about it.

Think about how often you encounter news stories or look up information on the Internet. Much of the time, the stories you read and the websites you visit will present information based on research. For example, during an election year, Americans may come across polling information in the media almost every day. Many online newspapers have science sections that include stories on the lat- est research. Entire websites are dedicated to psychology-related topics, such as treatments for autism, subliminal learning tapes, or advice for married couples. Magazines such as Scientific American, Men’s Health, and Parents summarize research for their readers. While some of the research—whether online or printed— is accurate and useful, some of it is dubious, and some is just plain wrong. How can you tell the good research information from the bad? Understanding research methods enables you to ask the appropriate questions so you can evaluate informa- tion correctly. Research methods skills apply not only to research studies but also to much of the other types of information you are likely to encounter in daily life.

FIGURE 1.1 Producers of research. As undergraduates, some psychology majors work alongside faculty members as producers of information.

 

 

8 CHAPTER 1 Psychology Is a Way of Thinking

Finally, being a smart consumer of research could be crucial to your future career. Even if you do not plan to be a researcher—if your goal is to be a social worker, a teacher, a sales representative, a human resources professional, an entrepreneur, or a parent—you will need to know how to interpret published research with a critical eye. Clinical psychologists, social workers, and family therapists must read research to know which therapies are the most effective. In fact, licensure in these helping professions requires knowing the research behind evidence-based treatments—that is, therapies that are supported by research. Teachers also use research to find out which teaching methods work best. And the business world runs on quantitative information: Research is used to predict what sales will be like in the future, what consumers will buy, and whether investors will take risks or lie low. Once you learn how to be a consumer of information—psychological or otherwise—you will use these skills constantly, no matter what job you have.

In this book, you will often see the phrase “interrogating information.” A con- sumer of research needs to know how to ask the right questions, determine the answers, and evaluate a study on the basis of those answers. This book will teach you systematic rules for interrogating research information.

The Benefits of Being a Good Consumer What do you gain by being a critical consumer of information? Imagine, for exam- ple, that you are a correctional officer at a juvenile detention center, and you watch a TV documentary about a crime-prevention program called Scared Straight. The program arranges for teenagers involved in the criminal justice system to visit prisons, where selected prisoners describe the stark, violent realities of prison life (Figure 1.2). The idea is that when teens hear about how tough it is in prison, they will be scared into the “straight,” law-abiding life. The program makes a lot

FIGURE 1.2 Scared straight. Although it makes intuitive sense that young people would be scared into good behavior by hearing from current prisoners, such intervention programs have actually been shown to cause an increase in criminal offenses.

 

 

9Research Producers, Research Consumers

of sense to you. You are considering starting a partnership between the residents of your detention center and the state prison system.

However, before starting the partnership, you decide to investigate the efficacy of the program by reviewing some research that has been conducted about it. You learn that despite the intuitive appeal of the Scared Straight approach, the program doesn’t work—in fact, it might even cause criminal activity to get worse! Several published articles have reported the results of randomized, controlled studies in which young adults were assigned to either a Scared Straight program or a control program. The researchers then collected criminal records for 6–12 months. None of the studies showed that Scared Straight attendees committed fewer crimes, and most studies found an increase in crime among participants in the Scared Straight programs, compared to the controls (Petrosino, Turpin-Petrosino, & Finckenauer, 2000). In one case, Scared Straight attendees had committed 20% more crimes than the control group.

At first, people considering such a program might think: If this program helps even one person, it’s worth it. However, we always need empirical evidence to test the efficacy of our interventions. A well-intentioned program that seems to make sense might actually be doing harm. In fact, if you investigate further, you’ll find that the U.S. Department of Justice officially warns that such programs are inef- fective and can harm youth, and the Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention Act of 1974 was amended to prohibit youth in the criminal justice system from interactions with adult inmates in jails and prisons.

Being a skilled consumer of information can inform you about other pro- grams that might work. For example, in your quest to become a better student, suppose you see this headline: “Mindfulness may improve test scores.” The prac- tice of mindfulness involves attending to the present moment, on purpose, with a nonjudgmental frame of mind (Kabat-Zinn, 2013). In a mindful state, people simply observe and let go of thoughts rather than elaborating on them. Could the practice of mindfulness really improve test scores? A study conducted by Michael Mrazek and his colleagues assigned people to take either a 2-week mindfulness training course or a 2-week nutrition course (Mrazek, Franklin, Philips, Baird, & Schooner, 2013). At the end of the training, only the people who had practiced mindfulness showed improved GRE scores (compared to their scores beforehand). Mrazek’s group hypothesized that mindfulness training helps people attend to an academic task without being distracted. They were bet- ter, it seemed, at controlling their minds from wandering. The research evidence you read about here appears to support the use of mindfulness for improving test scores.

By understanding the research methods and results of this study, you might be convinced to take a mindfulness-training course similar to the one used by Mrazek and his colleagues. And if you were a teacher or tutor, you might consider advising your students to practice some of the focusing techniques. (Chapter 10 returns to this example and explains why the Mrazek study stands up to interro- gation.) Your skills in research methods will help you become a better consumer of

 

 

10 CHAPTER 1 Psychology Is a Way of Thinking

studies like this one, so you can decide when the research supports some programs (such as mindfulness for study skills) but not others (such as Scared Straight for criminal behavior).

CHECK YOUR UNDERSTANDING

1. Explain what the consumer of research and producer of research roles have in common, and describe how they differ.

2. What kinds of jobs would use consumer-of-research skills? What kinds of jobs would use producer-of-research skills?

HOW SCIENTISTS APPROACH THEIR WORK Psychological scientists are identified not by advanced degrees or white lab coats; they are defined by what they do and how they think. The rest of this chapter will explain the fundamental ways psychologists approach their work. First, they act as empiricists in their investigations, meaning that they systematically observe the world. Second, they test theories through research and, in turn, revise their theories based on the resulting data. Third, they take an empirical approach to both applied research, which directly targets real-world problems, and basic research, which is intended to contribute to the general body of knowledge. Fourth, they go further: Once they have discovered an effect, scientists plan further research to test why, when, or for whom an effect works. Fifth, psychologists make their work public: They submit their results to journals for review and respond to the opinions of other scientists. Another aspect of making work public involves sharing findings of psy- chological research with the popular media, who may or may not get the story right.

Scientists Are Empiricists Empiricists do not base conclusions on intuition, on casual observations of their own experience, or on what other people say. Empiricism, also referred to as the empirical method or empirical research, involves using evidence from the senses (sight, hearing, touch) or from instruments that assist the senses (such as thermometers, timers, photographs, weight scales, and questionnaires) as the basis for conclusions. Empiricists aim to be systematic, rigorous, and to make their work independently verifiable by other observers or scientists.

Exploring Popular Assumptions

Give one example of a ‘folk-psychology’ claim that was not discussed in section this week. This can be something you have heard in your life, or one that you find on the Internet or popular media.  Explain why this claim is pseudoscientific, and not scientific.

Identifying Bias in Scientific Reporting

After discussing bias in class and lab this week, find a news article (about something scientific) that includes an example of one type of bias.

Answer the following:

a) Give the title and a link to the article you read.

b) Definition of the type of bias you identified.

c) Description of the example you found in a recent scientific news article.

d) Explain why this kind of bias is problematic.

d) Give a few possible ways that this bias could have been mitigated in your given example.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Forum 2 homework help

Forum 2 homework help

Week 2 Forumhttps://edge.apus.edu/messageforums-tool/images/collapse.gif?sakai.tool.placement.id=6011ae23-8668-4266-8df7-746a36db5923View Full Description

 

 

Topic FolderSocialization 0 unread of 0 messageshttps://edge.apus.edu/messageforums-tool/images/collapse.gif?sakai.tool.placement.id=6011ae23-8668-4266-8df7-746a36db5923View Full Description

Choose one of the following questions: 

1.  Do people adjust their behavior and presentation of the self to affect the opinion of others?  Would you say that we have different “social selves” that we present in different settings?  How does this relate to the concepts of front and back stage as well as the techniques of social actors discussed by Goffman?

2.  Do you think the mass media significantly contributes to the socialization process? When answering this question be sure to identify and describe the major agents of socialization in U.S. society today. Does the media have more influence over socialization than the other agents of social control? Why or why not?

3. Select a topic of contemporary relevance that is of interest to you (for example, poverty, juvenile delinquency, teen births, or racial neighborhood segregation). Briefly describe why you selected the topic.  Using what you learned in this chapter, create a simple research question about the topic. Match your research question to an appropriate research method and describe how as a sociologist you would conduct the research to answer your proposed question.

 

The Week 2 Forum meets the following course objectives:

  • Apply a sociological perspective to the social world
  • Analyze contemporary social issues using the sociological imagination and use sociological theories and concepts to analyze everyday life.
  • Recognize and define social structure and social interaction.
  • Demonstrate the ability to identify, locate, and retrieve information related to the topics in the course.
  • Describe the major research methods used in sociological research.

 

 

Instructions for all Forums:

Each week, learners will post one initial post per week.  This post must demonstrate comprehension of the course materials, the ability to apply that knowledge in the real world.  Learners will engage with the instructor and peers throughout the learning week.  To motivate engaged discussion, posts are expected to be on time with regular interaction throughout the week.  All posts should demonstrate college level writing skills. To promote vibrant discussion as we would in a face to face classroom, formatted citations and references are not required.  Quotes should not be used at all, or used sparingly.  If you quote a source quotation marks should be used and an APA formatted citation and reference provided.

 

 

 

Points

 

Exemplary (100%)

 

 

Accomplished (85%)

 

 

Developing (75%)

 

Beginning (65%)

 

Not Participating (0%)

 

Comprehension of course materials

 

4

Initial post demonstrates rich comprehension of course materials.  Detailed use of terminology or examples learned in class.  If post includes opinion, it is supported with evaluated evidence. Initial post demonstrates clear comprehension of course materials.  Use of terminology or examples learned in class. If post includes opinion, it is supported with evaluated evidence. Initial post demonstrates some comprehension of course materials.  Specific terminology or examples learned in class may be incorrect or incomplete.  Post may include some opinion without evaluated evidence. Initial post does not demonstrate comprehension of course materials.  Specific terminology or examples learned in class are not included.  Post is opinion based without evaluated evidence. No posting, post is off topic, post does not meet minimum criteria for demonstrating beginning level of comprehension. Post may be plagiarized, or use a high percentage of quotes that prevent demonstration of student’s comprehension.
Real world application of knowledge

 

2

Initial post demonstrates that the learner can creatively and uniquely apply the concepts and examples learned in class to a personal or professional experience from their life or to a current event. Initial post demonstrates that the learner can apply the concepts and examples learned in class to a  personal or professional experience from their life or to a current event. Initial post does not clearly demonstrate that the learner can apply the concepts and examples learned in class. Unclear link between the concepts and examples learned in class to personal or professional experience or to a current event. Initial post does not demonstrate that the learner can apply the concepts and examples learned in class. No link to a personal or professional experience or to a current event is made in the post. No posting, post is off topic, post does not meet minimum criteria for demonstrating beginning level of application. Post may be plagiarized, or use a high percentage of quotes that prevent demonstration of student’s ability to apply comprehension.
Active Forum Engagement and Presence

 3

Learner posts 4+ different days in the learning week.

 

Replies to at least one response from a classmate or instructor on the learner’s initial post to demonstrate the learner is reading and considering classmate responses to their ideas.

 

Posts two or more 100+ word responses to initial posts of classmates.  Posts motivate group discussion and contributes to the learning community by doing 2+ of the following:

  • offering advice or strategy
  • posing a question,
  • providing an alternative point-of-view,
  • acknowledging similar experiences
  • sharing a resource
Learner posts 3 different days in the learning week.

 

Posts two 100+ word responses to initial posts of classmates.  Posts motivate group discussion and contribute to the learning community by doing  2+ of the following:

 

  • offering advice or strategy
  • posing a question,
  • providing an alternative point-of-view,
  • acknowledging similar experiences
  • sharing a resource
Learner posts 2 different days in the learning week.

 

Posts one 100+ word response to initial post of classmate.  Post motivates group discussion and contributes to the learning community by doing 1 of the following:

 

  • offering advice or strategy
  • posing a question,
  • providing an alternative point-of-view,
  • acknowledging similar experiences
  • sharing a resource
Learner posts 1 day in the learning week.

 

Posts one 100+ word response to initial post of classmate.  Post does not clearly motivate group discussion or clearly contribute to the learning community.

 

Responses do not:

  • offering advice or strategy
  • posing a question,
  • providing an alternative point-of-view,
  • acknowledging similar experiences
  • sharing a resource
Learner posts 1 day in the learning week, or posts are not made during the learning week and therefore do not contribute to or enrich the weekly conversation.

 

No peer responses are made.  One or more peer responses of low quality (“good job, I agree”) may be made.

Writing skills

 1

Post is 250+ words.  All posts reflect widely accepted academic writing protocols like using capital letters, cohesive sentences, and no texting language. Dialogue is also polite and respectful of different points of view. Post is 250+ words.  The majority of posts reflect widely-accepted academic writing protocols like using capital letters, cohesive sentences, and no texting language. Dialogue is polite and respectful of different points of view. Post is 175+ words.  The majority of posts reflect widely-accepted academic writing protocols like using capital letters (“I am” not “i am”), cohesive sentences, and no texting language. Dialogue may not be respectful of different points of view. Post is 150+ words.  The majority of the forum communication ignores widely-accepted academic writing protocols like capital letters, cohesive sentences, and texting; Dialogue may not be respectful of different points of view. No posting, post is off topic and does not meet minimum criteria for demonstrating beginning level of comprehension.

 

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psych 665 – Week Six – Capstone Quiz

Psych 665 – Week Six – Capstone Quiz

University of Phoenix Material

 

Capstone Quiz

 

Answer the following multiple choice questions by highlighting the answer. There is one correct answer per question.

 

1.     Which historical perspective stated that psychologists should study the different components of the mind independently, because to understand how the conscious mind works, we must understand all of its individual parts completely?

 

a.     Structuralism

b.    Functionalism

c.     Behaviorism

d.    Gestalt

e.     Psychodynamic

 

2.     Edward Titchener used a method for studying the mind that became popular during the Structuralist period. The method, called _____, required trained participants to report their conscious mental experiences to the investigator. For example, if a person was angry, they would report all of their experiences during the time they were angry.

 

a.     empiricism

b.    functionalism

c.     contemplation

d.    introspection

e.     conscientiousness

 

3.     A potential problem with ___________ research is _____________.

 

a.     longitudinal; cohort differences

b.    cross-sectional; subject attrition

c.     cross-sectional; cohort differences

d.    longitudinal; random assignment

e.     cross-sectional; maturation

 

4.     Research by _____________  legitimized psychological science when it became the first psychological research presented as evidence to the United States Supreme Court.

 

a.     Muzafer Sherif

b.    Irving Janis

c.     Mamie Clark

d.    Phillip Zimbardo

e.     Kurt Lewin

 

5.     To determine whether changing one variable like education will produce changes in another like income, we must conduct _____________________ research.

 

a.     survey

b.    correlational

c.     experimental

d.    statistical

e.     basic

 

6.     Mary is studying the effect of high blood sugar on intelligence test performance. Which of these might be her hypothesis?

 

a.     People should not eat high sugar foods prior to IQ testing.

b.    High sugar foods increase energy and improve IQ test performance.

c.     People who eat high sugar foods before testing will have lower scores on an IQ test than people who do not.

d.    Roughly 75% of people had lower IQ test scores after eating high sugar snacks right before testing.

e.     Individuals should not be given high sugar snacks prior to IQ testing.

 

7.     Which of the following research methods would be most effective in demonstrating whether the presence of others improves our performance of a task?

 

a.     An experiment

b.    A correlational study

c.     A survey

d.    A field study

e.     An historical study

 

8.     Which of the following psychological studies would you expect to have similar results cross-culturally?

 

a.     Milgram’s study of obedience to authority

b.    Asch’s conformity study

c.     Study of the symptoms of mental illness

d.    Study of Maslow’s hierarchy of needs

e.     Study of parenting styles

 

9.     According to the APA Ethical Standards, psychologists must inform participants of the nature of the research; and that they are free to participate, decline to participate, or withdraw from the research at any time. These requirements, among others, are necessary to ensure the participants’ ______.

 

a.     anonymity

b.    risk level

c.     informed consent

d.    debriefing

e.     risk/benefit ratio

 

10.  Making an ethical decision involves

 

a.     simply applying a clear and definitive set of guidelines for ethical research

b.    deciding that an ethical decision is effective if it makes you happy

c.     identifying what ethical guidelines are relevant in a situation and what is at stake for all parties involved

d.    maintaining the anonymity of the researchers

e.     maintaining the anonymity of the participants

 

11.  According to the APA Ethical Standards, who is ultimately responsible for the ethical conduct of research done in psychology?

 

a.     The Institutional Review Board

b.    The individual researchers

c.     The sponsoring institution, such as the university

d.    The assistants who test the participants

e.     The funding agency

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Ethical Issues: Religion/Spirituality In Therapy Assignment Help

Ethical Issues: Religion/Spirituality In Therapy Assignment Help

HOT TOPIC

 

Ethical Issues for the Integration of Religion and Spirituality in Therapy

The past decade has witnessed increased attention to the importance of understanding and respecting client/patient spirituality and religiosity to psychological assessment and treatment, as well as recognition that religious and spiritual factors remain underexamined in research and practice (APA, 2007d). Advances in addressing the clinical relevance of faith in the lives of clients/patients have raised new ethical dilemmas rooted in theoretical models of personality historically isolated from client/patient faith beliefs, the paucity of research on the clinical benefits or harms of injecting faith concepts into treatment practices, group differences in religious practices and values, and individual differences in the salience of religion to mental health (Shafranske & Sperry, 2005; Tan, 2003).

The Secular–Theistic Therapy Continuum

Integration of religion/spirituality in therapy can be characterized on a secular–theistic continuum. Toward the secular end of the continuum are “religiously sensitive therapies” that blend traditional treatment approaches with sensitivity to the relationship of diverse religious/spiritual beliefs and behaviors to mental health. Midway on the continuum are “religiously accommodative therapies” that do not promote faith beliefs but, when clinically relevant, use religious/spiritual language and interventions consistent with clients’/patients’ faith values to foster mental health. Toward the other end of the continuum are “theistic therapies” that draw on psychologists’ own religious beliefs and use sacred texts and techniques (prayer, forgiveness, and meditation) to promote spiritual health.

The sections that follow highlight ethical challenges that emerge along all points of the secular–theistic therapy continuum.

Competence

All psychologists should have the training and experience necessary to identify when a mental health problem is related to or grounded in religious beliefs ( Standards 2.01 b, Boundaries of Competence, and 2.03, Maintaining Competence; see also Bartoli, 2007; W. B. Johnson, 2004; Plante, 2007; Raiya & Pargament, 2010; Yarhouse & Tan, 2005). Personal faith and religious experience are neither sufficient nor necessary for competence (Gonsiorek, Richards, Pargament, & McMinn, 2009). There is no substitute for familiarity with the foundational empirical and professional mental health knowledge base and treatment techniques. While personal familiarity with a client’s/patient’s religious affiliation can be informative, religious/spiritual therapeutic competencies for mental health treatment include

 

· understanding how religion presents itself in mental health and psychopathology;

· self-awareness of religious bias that may impair therapeutic effectiveness, including awareness that being a member of a faith tradition is not evidence of expertise in the integration of religion/spirituality into mental health treatment;

· techniques to assess and treat clinically relevant religious/spiritual beliefs and emotional reactions; and

· knowledge of data on mental health effectiveness of religious imagery, prayer, or other religious techniques.

Collaboration With Clergy. Collaborations with clergy can help inform psychologists about the origins of the client’s beliefs, demonstrate respect for the client’s religion, and avoid trespassing into theological domains by increasing the probability that a client’s incorrect religious interpretations will be addressed appropriately within his or her faith community (W. B. Johnson, Redley, & Nielson, 2000; Richards & Bergin, 2005;  Standard 3.09 , Cooperation With Other Professionals). When cooperation with clergy will be clinically helpful to a client/patient, psychologists should

 

· obtain written permission/authorization from the client/patient to speak with a specific identified member of the clergy,

· share only information needed for both to be of optimal assistance to the client/patient ( Standard 4.04 , Minimizing Intrusions on Privacy),

· discuss with the clergy where roles might overlap (e.g., family counseling, sexual issues), and

· determine ways in which the client/patient can get the best assistance.

Avoiding Secular–Theistic Bias

Psychologists must ensure that their professional and personal biases do not interfere with the provision of appropriate and effective mental health services for persons of diverse religious beliefs (Principle D: Justice and Principle E: Respect for People’s Rights and Dignity;  Standards 2.06 , Personal Problems and Conflicts, and 3.01, Unfair Discrimination).

Disputation or Unquestioned Acceptance of Client/Patient Faith Beliefs. Trivializing or disputing religious values and beliefs can undermine the goals of therapy by threatening those aspects of life that some clients/patients hold sacred, that provide supportive family and community connections, and that form an integral part of their identity (Pargament, Murray-Swank, Magyar, & Ano, 2005;  Standard 3.04 , Avoiding Harm). Similarly, some religious coping styles can be deleterious to client/patient mental health (Sood, Fisher, & Sulmasy, 2006), and uncritical acceptance of theistic beliefs, when they indicate misunderstandings or distortions of religious teachings and values, can undercut treatment goals by reinforcing maladaptive ways of thinking or by ignoring signs of psychopathology. In addition, psychologists should not assume that religious or spiritual beliefs are static and be prepared to help clients/patients identify changes reflecting spiritual maturity positively tied to treatment goals (Knapp, Lemoncelli, & VandeCreek, 2010). To identify if clients’/patients’ religious beliefs are having a deleterious effect on their mental health, psychologists should explore whether their beliefs (a) create or exacerbate clinical distress, (b) provide a way to avoid reality and responsibility, (c) lead to self-destructive behavior, or (d) create false expectations of God (W. B. Johnson et al., 2000). When appropriate, psychologists should consider consulting with clergy to determine if a clients’/patients’ religious beliefs are distortions or misconceptions of religious doctrine.

Imposing Religious Values

Using the therapist’s authority to indoctrinate clients/patients to the psychologists’ religious beliefs violates their value autonomy and exploits their vulnerability to coercion (Principle E: Respect for People’s Rights and Dignity;  Standard 3.08 , Exploitative Relationships). When clients/patients are grappling with decisions in areas in which religious and secular moral perspectives may conflict (e.g., divorce, sexual orientation, abortion, acceptance of transfusions, end-of-life decisions), therapy needs to distinguish between those religious values that have positive or destructive influences on each individual client’s/patient’s mental health—not the religious or secular values of the psychologist. Professional license to practice psychology demands that psychologists provide competent professional services and does not give them license to preach (Plante, 2007). Psychologists should guard against discussing religious doctrine when it is irrelevant to the clients’/patients’ mental health needs (Richards & Bergin, 2005).

Confusing Religious Values With Psychological Diagnoses. The revised Guidelines for Psychological Practice With Lesbian, Gay and Bisexual Clients (APA, 2012d) encourages psychologists to consider the influences of religion and spirituality in the lives of lesbian, gay, and bisexual specifically and transgender and questioning clients in general. The linking of religious values and psychotherapies involving LGBT clients/patients has drawn a considerable amount of public attention. Spiritually sensitive, accommodative, and theistic therapies have a lot to offer LGBT clients/patients (Lease, Horne, & Noffsinger-Frazier, 2005). LGBT persons vary in their religious backgrounds and the extent to which it affects their psychological well-being. Ethical problems arise, however, when psychologists confuse a client’s/patient’s conflicted feelings about their sexual orientation and religious values with psychological diagnoses. Such ethical challenges have raised considerable professional dialogue as they relate to the application of conversion therapies to alter sexual orientation.

All major professional mental health organizations have affirmed that homosexuality is not a mental disorder ( www.apa.org/pi/lgbc/publications/justthefacts.html#2 ). In addition, to date, empirical data dispute the effectiveness of conversion/reparative therapies aimed at changing sexual orientation ( www.Psychology.org.au/Assets/Files/reparative_therapy.pdf ). Psychologists who offer such therapies to LGBT clients/patients risk violating  Standard 2.04 , Bases for Scientific and Professional Judgments. Moreover, when psychologists offer “cures” for homosexuality, they falsely imply that there is established knowledge in the profession that LGBT sexual orientation is a mental disorder. This, in turn, may deprive clients/patients of exploring internalized reactions to a hostile society and risks perpetuating societal prejudices and stereotypes (Cramer, Golom, LoPresto, & Kirkley, 2008; Haldeman, 1994, 2004; Principle A: Beneficence and Nonmaleficence; Principle B: Fidelity and Responsibility; and Principle D: Justice;  Standard 3.04 , Avoiding Harm). In addition, when psychologists base their diagnosis and treatment on religious doctrines that view homosexual behavior as a “sin,” they can be in violation of  Standard 9.01 , Bases for Assessments, and may be practicing outside the boundaries of their profession.

Multiple Relationships

Multiple relationship challenges arise when clergy who have doctoral degrees in psychology provide mental health services to congregants or nonclergy psychologists who treat members of their faith communities ( Standard 3.05 , Multiple Relationships).

Clergy–Psychologists. Clergy–psychologists providing therapy for members of their faith over whom they may have ecclesiastical authority should take steps to ensure they and their clients/patients are both aware of and respect the boundaries between their roles as a psychologist and as a religious leader. Distinguishing role functions becomes particularly important in addressing issues of confidentiality. Psychologists and clergy have different legal and professional obligations when it comes to mandated reporting of abuse and ethically permitted disclosures of information to protect clients/patients and others from harm ( Standard 4.05 , Disclosures).

Therapists at all points along the secular–theistic continuum who share the faith beliefs of clients/patients or work with fellow congregants must take steps to ensure that clients do not misperceive them as having religious or ecclesiastical authority and understand that the psychologists do not act on behalf of the church or its leaders (Gubi, 2001; Richards & Potts, 1995). This may be especially challenging for nonclergy religious psychologists working in faith-based environments (Sanders, Swenson, & Schneller, 2011). Psychologists also need to take steps to ensure that their knowledge of their joint faith community does not interfere with their objectivity and that clients/patients feel safe disclosing and exploring concerns about religion or behaviors that might ostracize them from this community.

Fee-for-Service Quandaries. While psychologists can discuss spiritual issues in therapy, when services are provided as a licensed psychologist eligible for third-party payments, the primary focus must be psychological (Plante, 2007). A focus on religious/spiritual rather than therapeutic goals may risk inappropriately charging third-party payors for nonmental health services not covered by insurance policies (Tan, 2003; see also Principle C: Integrity;  Standard 6.04 , Fees and Financial Arrangements). Clergy and nonclergy psychologists practicing theistic therapies may find it difficult to clearly differentiate in reports to third-party payors those goals and therapeutic techniques that are accepted mental health practices and those that are spiritually based. In most instances clergy–psychologists should encourage their congregants to seek mental health services from other providers in the community and refrain from encouraging their congregants to see them for fee-for-service therapy ( Standard 3.06 , Conflict of Interest). When clergy or nonclergy psychologists provide spiritual counseling free of charge in religious settings, they should clarify they are counseling in their ecclesiastical role and that content will be specific to pastoral issues (Richards & Bergin, 2005).

Informed Consent

The role of religion/spirituality in clients’/patients’ worldview may determine their willingness to participate in therapies along the secular–theistic continuum. Some may find the interjection of religion into therapy discomforting or coercive, while others may find the absence of religion from therapy alienating.

When scientific or professional knowledge indicate that discussion of religion may be essential for effective treatment ( Standards 2.01 b, Boundaries of Competence; 2.04, Bases for Scientific and Professional Judgments), informed consent discussions can help the client/patient and psychologist identify and limit for treatment those religious beliefs and practices that facilitate or interfere with treatment goals (Rosenfeld, 2011; Shumway & Waldo, 2012). In some contexts, it may be ethically appropriate to discuss the risks involved in exploration of the client’s religious beliefs, including loss of current coping mechanisms, stress produced by self-questioning of religious beliefs, and diminished capacity to seek support from one’s religious community (Rosenfeld, 2011). The goal of such discussions is to enhance the therapeutic alliance and treatment context through client–therapist mutual understanding and respect.

When treatments diverge from established psychological practice, clients/patients have a right to consider this information in their consent decisions. Consequently, informed consent for theistic therapies should explain the religious doctrine and values upon which their treatment is based, the religious methods that will be employed (e.g., prayers, reading of scripture, forgiveness), and the relative emphasis on spiritual versus mental health goals. In addition, since theistic therapies are relatively new and currently lack empirical evidence or disciplinary consensus regarding their use (Plante & Sherman, 2001; Richards & Bergin, 2005), psychologists practicing these therapies should consider whether informed consent requirements for “treatments for which generally recognized techniques and procedures have not been established,” described in  Standard 10.01b , apply.

Conclusion

There is a welcome increase in research examining the positive and negative influences of religious beliefs and practices on mental health and the clinical outcomes of treatment approaches along the secular–theistic therapy continuum. Ethical commitment to do what is right for each client/patient and well-informed approaches to treatment will reduce, but not eliminate, ethical challenges that will continue to emerge as scientific and professional knowledge advances. Psychologists conducting psychotherapy with individuals of diverse religious backgrounds and values will need to keep abreast of new knowledge and ethical guidelines that will emerge, continuously monitor the consequences of spirituality and religiously sensitive treatment decisions on client/patient well-being, and have the flexibility and sensitivity to religious contexts, role responsibilities, and client/patient expectations required for effective ethical decision making.

 

 

Reference:

 

 

Fisher, C. B. (20120904). Decoding the Ethics Code: A Practical Guide for Psychologists, 3rd Edition. [MBS Direct]. Retrieved from https://mbsdirect.vitalsource.com/#/books/978145228587

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology homework help

Psychology homework help

Question 1    1.    Which of the following is NOT a tip for choosing a co-leader?

a.  Share all aspects of planning and running the group.

b.  Work with leaders with whom you can have a cooperative and honest relationship.

c.  Find people who are the same age as you.

d.  Interview all prospective co-leaders.      5 points

Question 2    1.    The curative factor that helps people feel that they are not alone or isolated with the “uniqueness” of their problems is _____

a.  linking.

b.  altruism.

c.  universality.

d.  congruence.      5 points

Question 3    1.    “Tandeming” is _____

a.  a group skill.

b.  a form of member resistance.

c.  a problematic co-leader communication pattern.

d.  a curative factor only available in co-facilitated groups.      5 points

Question 4    1.    Adlerian theory focuses on human beings in _____

a.  a vocational context.

b.  a social context.

c.  a psychosexual context.

d.  an academic context.      5 points

Question 5    1.    Gestalt groups focus on _____

a.  historical exploration.

b.  family systems.

c.  here-and-now interactions.

d.  listening skills.      5 points

Question 6    1.    “Empowerment” primarily deals with the issue of _____

a.  primacy.

b.  linkage.

c.  self-advocacy.

d.  justice modeling.      5 points

Question 7    1.    The “curative factors” were established by _____

a.  Homeyer.

b.  Siepmann.

c.  Morrison.

d.  Yalom.      5 points

Question 8    1.    One distinct advantage of group over individual counseling is the ability to _____

a.  work on goals.

b.  terminate effectively.

c.  choose a theory.

d.  discover others.      5 points

Question 9    1.    If the group leader moves out of an ethnocentric lens and more fully recognizes the richness of the cultural elements of each member, which dimension is being experienced?

a.  Justice potential.

b.  Creativity.

c.  Multi-leveled linking.

d.  Multicultural integration.      5 points

Question 10    1.    In developing your own theory, you should find a congruence between _____

a.  your beliefs and the group’s beliefs.

b.  your beliefs and your professor’s beliefs.

c.  the philosophy of the theory and the rules of your site.

d.  the philosophy of the theory and your beliefs.      5 points

Question 11    1.    The group providing an immediate, first-hand opportunity for the group members to change their perceptions and to practice more mature social living is an example of _____

a.  cohesion.

b.  reality testing lab.

c.  linking.

d.  extrapolation.      5 points

Question 12    1.    Pregroup interviews can be used to _____

a.  orient the member to the group.

b.  go over group rules.

c.  assess if the member is a good fit for the group.

d.  all of the above.      5 points

Question 13    1.      Which of the following is a possible disadvantage of group counseling?

a.  Higher cost.

b.  More one-on-one focus.

c.    Some people do not feel safe in a group.

d.  Lack of real learning.      5 points       Q

uestion 14    1.    The primary problematic elements of dual relationships involve _____

a.  trust and mistrust.

b.  social justice and oppression.

c.  power differential and exploitation.

d.  role shift and role combining.      5 points

Question 15    1.     Confidentiality in group work _____

a.   cannot be guaranteed.

b.   can be guaranteed.

c.   applies only to counseling groups.

d.   is protected by law.      5 points

Question 16    1.    Two common types of co-leader incompatibility include _____

a.  political and financial.

b.  theoretical and personality.

c.  religious and spiritual.

d.  racial and family of origin.      5 points

Question 17    1.    This leadership style is egalitarian in orientation and participation by all group members in establishing goals is encouraged.

a.  Democratic.

b.  Assertive.

c.  Process Observer.

d.  Explorer.      5 points

Question 18    1.    Advertising and networking are two critical pieces of which step of group creation?

a.  Reaching out.

b.  Forming.

c.  Marketing and recruiting.

d.  Setting up.      5 points

Question 19    1.    One benefit of consulting the literature about your group idea is that it _____

a.  prevents you “reinventing the wheel.”

b.  improves your reading skill.

c.  expands your profit margin.

d.  covers your ethical obligation.      5 points

Question 20    1.    The division of the American Counseling Association that focuses on group work is _____

a.  American Group Work Association.

b.  Association of Group Work.

c.  American Group Society.

d.  Association for the Specialists in Group Work.      5 points

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Applied Behavioral Analysis 2

Applied Behavioral Analysis 2

Resource: How to Make a Graph Using Microsoft Excel

The Unit 6 Assignment requires you to apply the theories, concepts, and research that you have covered so far this term to a hypothetical case study. Your answers to the questions and completed graph should consist of information from the text and supplemental readings.You also may use sources from the Kaplan library or other credible Internet sources, but your primary sources should be the readings assigned for the course.

Read each Case Study and answer the questions below. You will need to write 2–3 typed pages for each case in order to address all required parts of the project.Answers to the questions should be typed in an APA formatted Word document, double-spaced in 12-point font and submitted to the Dropbox.

Your final paper must be your original work; plagiarism will not be tolerated. Be sure to review the Syllabus in terms of what constitutes plagiarism.Please make sure to provide proper credit for those sources used in your case study analysis in proper APA format. Please see the APA Quick Reference for any questions related to APA citations. You must credit authors when you:

Summarize a concept, theory or research
Use direct quotes from the text or articles
Read Case Study 1: Martin

Martin, a behavior analyst, is working with Sara, a 14-year-old girl with severe developmental delays who exhibits self-injurious behavior (SIB). Sara’s target behavior is defined as pulling her hair, biting her arm and banging her head against the wall. After conducting a functional analysis, Martin decided to employ an intervention program consisting of differential reinforcement of other (DRO) desired behavior. Martin collected data on Sara’s SIB before and during the intervention. Below is a depiction of the data that Martin collected:

Sara’s Frequency of SIB

BASELINE Occurrences DRO Occurrences
22 5
25 5
27 3
26 2

 

Address the following questions, and complete the following requirements:

Create a basic line graph using Microsoft Excel, to be included in your Word document. The graph should depict the data provided in this case study. You should only need to create one graph, with SIB depicted, both in baseline and in intervention.
What type of research design did Martin employ when working with Sara? What is an advantage and a disadvantage of using this research design?
According to the data in the graph, was the intervention that Martin selected effective in modifying Sara’s self-injurious behavior?
Martin had considered using an ABAB reversal design when working with Sara. What are some ethical implications of selecting a reversal design when working with the type of behavior problems that Sara was exhibiting?
Martin’s supervisor requested a graph of the data he collected when working with Sara. Why are graphs useful in evaluating behavior change?
Discuss how a graph demonstrates a functional relationship. Identify whether the graph that you created using the data provided in this section depicts a functional relationship.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Spas 1 Psychology homework help

Spas 1 Psychology homework help

PSY 520 SPSS Assignment 1

 

Before you begin the assignment:

 

• Read Chapter 8 in your Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics textbook.

• Review the video tutorial for an overview of conducting multiple regression in SPSS.

• Download and open the Popularity SPSS data set.

 

An overview of the data set:

 

This data set represents hypothetical data from a study that examined how well some core personality traits predict a person’s level of popularity. Personality was measured using the “Big 5,” which is a very commonly used measure of personality. In fact, a Big 5 personality scale was included in the Module Two discussion.

 

Here is some more information about the variables in this hypothetical data set:

 

• Number: This is the ID number of the participant

• Sex: Participants’ sex, with “1” standing for male and “2” standing for female

• Age: College year of the participant, with “1” standing for freshman, “2” standing for sophomore, etc.

• Popularity: Popularity measured with a questionnaire that could range from 0 to 100, with higher numbers indicating more popularity

• Extroversion: A Big 5 trait indicating level of sociability. Scores range from 1 to 5, with high numbers indicating extroverted and low numbers indicating introverted

• Agreeableness: A Big 5 trait indicating level of interpersonal warmth and friendliness. Scores range from 1 to 5, with high numbers indicating warmth and low numbers indicating coldness towards others

• Conscientiousness: A Big 5 trait indicating level of self-control and responsibility. Scores range from 1 to 5, with high numbers indicating high conscientiousness and low numbers indicating low conscientiousness

• Neuroticism: A Big 5 trait indicating level of anxiety and emotional stability. Scores range from 1 to 5, with high numbers indicating high neuroticism and low numbers indicating low neuroticism

• Openness: A Big 5 trait indicating level of willingness to try new things and creativity. Scores range from 1 to 5, with high numbers indicating high open-mindedness and low numbers indicating closed-mindedness

 

Questions:

 

1) Describe in your own words what type of research situations call for a researcher to use a multiple regression analysis.

 

Type answer below:

 

 

 

2a) Run a basic correlation of matrix for the Popularity, Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness variables.

 

Paste output below (Read carefully: The best way to do this is to select “Copy Special” when copying from the SPSS output. Then select image as a format to copy. When pasting in Word, select Paste Special, choose a picture format, and then resize the image so it fits the screen):

 

 

 

2b) Based on these results, which personality variables are significantly correlated with Popularity?

 

Type answer below:

 

 

 

3a) Conduct a multiple regression analysis using Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness as predictors of Popularity.

 

Paste the output below:

 

 

 

3b) Which variables are significant predictors of Popularity? Compare and contrast the results from the multiple regression analysis to the basic correlation results from question 2b.

 

Type your answer below:

 

 

 

3c) What is the R-squared of this model, and what does it tell us about how well this model predicts Popularity?

 

Type your answer below:

 

 

 

3d) Write the results of the multiple regression in APA style. For help, refer to the Regression section in this document.

 

Type answer below:

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!