Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education

Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education

Epilepsy is a neurological disorder that affects millions of people worldwide, transcending age, gender, and ethnicity. Characterized by recurrent seizures, epilepsy poses significant challenges to those affected and their families, impacting various aspects of daily life. Despite its prevalence, epilepsy remains widely misunderstood, leading to misconceptions and stigmatization. Therefore, a comprehensive understanding of epilepsy, including its causes, signs and symptoms, diagnosis, treatment regimens, and patient education, is essential for effective management and support. This paper aims to explore the multifaceted aspects of epilepsy, shedding light on its complexities while providing insights into current practices for diagnosis, treatment, and patient care. By delving into the intricacies of epilepsy, we can foster greater awareness, empathy, and support for individuals living with this condition, ultimately working towards a more inclusive and informed society. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

(Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Causes

Epilepsy can arise from various factors, both intrinsic and extrinsic to the central nervous system. One significant factor contributing to epilepsy is genetic predisposition. Certain genetic mutations or abnormalities can increase the likelihood of developing epilepsy. These genetic factors may influence the structure and function of neuronal networks, predisposing individuals to abnormal electrical activity in the brain, which can lead to seizures.

Furthermore, epilepsy can result from acquired brain injuries or abnormalities. Traumatic brain injury due to accidents or sports-related injuries can disrupt neuronal pathways and lead to epileptic seizures. Similarly, stroke, which causes damage to brain tissue due to lack of blood flow, can result in epilepsy. Brain tumors, particularly those affecting regions responsible for regulating neuronal activity, can also trigger epileptic seizures.

Infections of the central nervous system, such as meningitis or encephalitis, can lead to inflammation and scarring of brain tissue, disrupting normal neuronal function and increasing the risk of epilepsy. Developmental disorders affecting brain structure, such as cortical dysplasia or neurofibromatosis, can also predispose individuals to epilepsy.

Additionally, prenatal and perinatal factors can contribute to the development of epilepsy. Conditions such as maternal drug use, infections, or complications during childbirth can affect fetal brain development, increasing the risk of epilepsy later in life.

In some cases, the cause of epilepsy remains unknown, termed idiopathic epilepsy. While the underlying cause may not be identified, idiopathic epilepsy is believed to have a genetic component, indicating a complex interplay of genetic and environmental factors in its development.

Overall, epilepsy is a heterogeneous disorder with a multifactorial etiology. Understanding the various causes of epilepsy is crucial for tailoring treatment approaches and providing appropriate support to individuals affected by this condition. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Signs and Symptoms

Epilepsy presents with a diverse array of signs and symptoms, often varying among individuals and across different types of seizures. The hallmark of epilepsy is the occurrence of recurrent seizures, which result from abnormal electrical activity in the brain.

Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education

Seizures can manifest in various forms, ranging from subtle changes in awareness to dramatic convulsions. Some individuals may experience focal seizures, which originate in a specific area of the brain and may be associated with sensory or motor disturbances. These seizures can cause altered consciousness, repetitive movements, or unusual sensations such as tingling or visual disturbances.

Generalized seizures involve widespread neuronal activity and can lead to loss of consciousness and uncontrolled movements. These seizures may present as tonic-clonic seizures, characterized by stiffening (tonic phase) followed by rhythmic jerking (clonic phase) of the limbs. Absence seizures, another type of generalized seizure, are brief episodes of staring or transient loss of awareness, often mistaken for daydreaming.

Other signs and symptoms of epilepsy may include auras, which are sensory disturbances or unusual sensations that precede a seizure, providing a warning sign for some individuals. Postictal symptoms, occurring after a seizure, can include confusion, fatigue, headache, or muscle soreness.

In addition to seizures, epilepsy can impact various aspects of daily life and may be associated with comorbidities such as cognitive impairments, mood disorders, or sleep disturbances. These comorbidities can further affect quality of life and require comprehensive management strategies.

It is essential for individuals with epilepsy and their caregivers to recognize the signs and symptoms of seizures, as timely intervention and appropriate management can help mitigate their impact. Furthermore, understanding the diverse manifestations of epilepsy promotes empathy and support for those affected by this condition. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Etiology

Epilepsy is a complex disorder with a multifaceted etiology, involving a combination of genetic, developmental, and acquired factors. Genetic predisposition plays a significant role in the development of epilepsy, with certain genetic mutations or variations increasing susceptibility to abnormal neuronal activity in the brain. Family history of epilepsy or related neurological conditions can also contribute to an individual’s risk.

Developmental abnormalities in brain structure or function can predispose individuals to epilepsy. Conditions such as cortical dysplasia, in which the brain’s outer layer fails to develop properly, or neurofibromatosis, characterized by the growth of tumors in the nervous system, can disrupt normal neuronal networks and increase the likelihood of seizures.

Acquired factors, such as brain injury or infection, can also precipitate epilepsy. Traumatic brain injury resulting from accidents or physical trauma can cause structural damage to the brain, disrupting neuronal pathways and increasing the risk of seizures. Similarly, infections of the central nervous system, such as meningitis or encephalitis, can lead to inflammation and scarring of brain tissue, further contributing to epileptogenesis.

Prenatal and perinatal factors are also implicated in the etiology of epilepsy. Maternal factors such as drug use, infections, or complications during pregnancy can affect fetal brain development, increasing the risk of epilepsy later in life. Similarly, birth complications or perinatal injuries can impact brain function and predispose individuals to epilepsy.

In some cases, the exact cause of epilepsy remains unknown, termed idiopathic epilepsy. While the underlying mechanisms may not be fully understood, idiopathic epilepsy is believed to have a genetic component, highlighting the complex interplay of genetic and environmental factors in its etiology.

Understanding the diverse etiological factors contributing to epilepsy is essential for tailoring management strategies and providing appropriate support to individuals affected by this condition. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Pathophysiology

Epilepsy is characterized by abnormal neuronal activity in the brain, leading to recurrent seizures. The pathophysiology of epilepsy involves complex interactions within neuronal networks, resulting in disruptions to normal electrical signaling and synchronization.

At the cellular level, epilepsy is associated with alterations in ion channel function and neurotransmitter release. Abnormalities in ion channels, which regulate the flow of ions across neuronal membranes, can lead to hyperexcitability or hypersynchrony of neuronal firing, predisposing individuals to seizures. Disruptions in neurotransmitter systems, such as gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and glutamate, further contribute to abnormal neuronal activity in epilepsy.

Structural abnormalities in the brain can also play a role in the pathophysiology of epilepsy. Conditions such as cortical dysplasia, hippocampal sclerosis, or brain tumors can disrupt normal neuronal architecture, leading to focal areas of hyperexcitability and seizure generation.

Additionally, abnormalities in network connectivity and synchronization contribute to the generation and propagation of seizures in epilepsy. Disruptions in the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neuronal circuits can result in excessive neuronal firing and synchronization, leading to seizure activity.

The pathophysiology of epilepsy is dynamic and multifactorial, involving a complex interplay of genetic, developmental, and acquired factors. While the exact mechanisms underlying epileptogenesis remain incompletely understood, advances in neuroimaging, electrophysiology, and molecular biology have provided insights into the complex interactions within neuronal networks.

Understanding the pathophysiological mechanisms of epilepsy is crucial for developing targeted treatment strategies aimed at modulating neuronal excitability and preventing seizure generation. By elucidating the underlying mechanisms driving epileptogenesis, researchers aim to identify novel therapeutic targets and improve outcomes for individuals living with epilepsy. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

DSM-5 Diagnosis

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), provides criteria for diagnosing epilepsy based on the presence of recurrent seizures and their specific characteristics. According to the DSM-5, epilepsy is classified as a neurological disorder characterized by the occurrence of at least two unprovoked seizures occurring more than 24 hours apart.

The DSM-5 outlines specific criteria for the diagnosis of epilepsy, including the documentation of seizure types, frequency, and associated features. Detailed history-taking and thorough neurological examination are essential for accurately diagnosing epilepsy and distinguishing it from other conditions that may present with seizure-like symptoms.

The DSM-5 emphasizes the importance of differentiating between various types of seizures, including focal onset seizures, generalized onset seizures, and unknown onset seizures. Detailed descriptions of seizure semiology, including motor, sensory, autonomic, and behavioral features, aid in categorizing seizures according to their origin and characteristics.

In addition to clinical evaluation, diagnostic tests such as electroencephalography (EEG), magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), and blood tests may be performed to further evaluate the underlying cause of epilepsy and assess the extent of brain involvement.

The DSM-5 also recognizes the importance of considering comorbid psychiatric conditions in individuals with epilepsy, as they may significantly impact treatment outcomes and overall quality of life. Common comorbidities include mood disorders, anxiety disorders, and cognitive impairments.

Overall, the DSM-5 provides a comprehensive framework for diagnosing epilepsy, emphasizing the importance of detailed history-taking, thorough clinical evaluation, and consideration of comorbid conditions. Accurate diagnosis and classification of epilepsy facilitate appropriate treatment selection and management, ultimately improving outcomes for individuals living with this condition. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Treatment Regimens

Effective management of epilepsy requires a multifaceted approach that encompasses pharmacological interventions, lifestyle modifications, and patient education. The primary goal of treatment is to achieve seizure control while minimizing side effects and improving overall quality of life for individuals living with epilepsy.

Pharmacological Interventions: Antiepileptic medications (AEDs) are the cornerstone of epilepsy treatment. These medications work by stabilizing neuronal activity in the brain and reducing the likelihood of seizure occurrence. The selection of AEDs is tailored to the individual’s seizure type, frequency, and potential side effects. It is essential for patients to adhere to their prescribed medication regimen consistently to optimize seizure control. Regular follow-up appointments with healthcare providers are necessary to monitor treatment response and adjust medication dosages as needed.

Lifestyle Modifications: In addition to medication, lifestyle modifications can play a significant role in managing epilepsy. Strategies such as maintaining a regular sleep schedule, avoiding triggers such as alcohol or recreational drugs, managing stress through relaxation techniques, and adhering to a healthy diet may help reduce seizure frequency and improve overall well-being. Regular exercise, while promoting physical health, should be pursued cautiously, taking into account individual seizure triggers and safety precautions.

Patient Education: Patient education is paramount in empowering individuals with epilepsy to actively participate in their treatment and self-management. Key components of patient education include understanding epilepsy and its causes, recognizing seizure triggers, knowing how to respond during a seizure episode, and adhering to medication regimens. Patients should also be educated about potential side effects of AEDs and strategies for managing them effectively. Furthermore, patients and their families should receive training in administering first aid during seizures and be knowledgeable about when to seek emergency medical assistance.

Epilepsy management requires a comprehensive approach that integrates pharmacological interventions, lifestyle modifications, and patient education. By addressing seizure control, minimizing side effects, and promoting self-management strategies, individuals living with epilepsy can achieve better outcomes and enhanced quality of life. Empowering patients through education and support fosters autonomy and resilience in managing their condition, ultimately leading to improved overall well-being and functional independence. Ongoing research and advancements in treatment modalities continue to expand therapeutic options and improve outcomes for individuals affected by epilepsy. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

Conclusion

Epilepsy is a complex neurological disorder that requires a multifaceted approach to management. By exploring its causes, signs and symptoms, etiology, pathophysiology, DSM-5 diagnosis, treatment regimens, and patient education, we gain valuable insights into this condition. The updated discussion on treatment regimens emphasizes the importance of personalized pharmacological interventions, lifestyle modifications, and patient education in achieving optimal seizure control and enhancing quality of life for individuals with epilepsy. Furthermore, the emphasis on patient education underscores the significance of empowering patients to actively participate in their treatment and self-management. Through ongoing research and advancements in treatment modalities, there is hope for continued improvement in outcomes and support for those living with epilepsy. By fostering greater awareness, empathy, and support, we can work towards a more inclusive and informed society that better understands and supports individuals affected by epilepsy. (Nursing Paper Example on Understanding Epilepsy: Causes, Symptoms, Diagnosis, Treatment, and Patient Education)

References

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK430765/

 

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology CST 110 Assignment Racist Blog

Psychology CST 110 Assignment Racist Blog

CST 110-9YM: INTRODUCTION TO COMMUNICATION (3 credits) – spring 2024 NORTHERN VIRGINIA COMMUNITY COLLEGE – MANASSAS CAMPUS

Friday – 9:35 -10:55pm Virtual

 

Instructor: Dr. Gerald Powell * Email : gpowell@nvcc.edu Location: MH 411-29 Engagement Hours: Face to Face & Virtual M,W 8:00-9:00 am & 11-12; T&TR Virtual Only Fri, 8:00-9:00am.

Office Phone: 703/530-3557

*The best mode of communication

 

 

Course Description and Objectives

 

CST 110 examines the elements affecting speech communication at the individual, small group, and public communication levels with emphasis on the practice of communication at each level. Upon completion of this course, the student should be able to:

 

Critical Thinking

• Analyze principles of communication

• Articulate the benefits and challenges of communication in present-day society or in their own lives

 

Civic Engagement

• Critically analyze how communication skills can be used to manage conflicts and improve relational

outcomes

• Analyze the relationship between communication and diversity

 

Professional Readiness

• Orally communicate ideas to a listener with verbal and nonverbal fluency

• Apply specific communication concepts or theories to their personal and professional lives

Written Communication

• Analyze the various components of a public presentation and develop a written outline and/or

manuscript

 

Fundamentals of Communication:

• Explain the role of ethics in various communication situations

• Identify, describe, and compare characteristics of language and explain denotative and connotative

meaning

• Identify types of listening and non-listening behaviors and demonstrate appropriate responses in

listening situations

• Compare/contrast various communication models

• Define and describe types of nonverbal communication and apply this knowledge to real-life situations

• Illustrate an understanding of the perception process, and make connections between self-concept and

interactions with others

• Analyze, select, and explain various aspects of communication situations in relation to appropriate goals,

objectives, and techniques.

 

 

Major Topics to be Included Fundamentals of Communication:

Interpersonal Communication Small Group Communication Public Speaking Media Literacy/Mass Media

 

 

 

Text & Materials

Joseph M. ValenzanoIII, Melissa A. Broekelman-Post,Erin Sahlstein Parcell , Communication Pathways 2nd edition (Fountainhead 2019). Two Three Scantron forms must also be purchased for taking required tests.

 

 

Required Assignments and Tests

(Note: Copies of all assignments are available on the CST 110 course Canvas site).

 

The assignments listed below are due on the date noted in the daily schedule that is included with this course syllabus. There is no “late” submission of assignments or “make-up” of tests except under extreme circumstances that are fully discussed with the instructor.

 

 

1)Two Exams: (100 points each – total 200 points)

Three objective exams, each representing 100 points, covering information presented in the text and class lectures. Exam review study guides can be found under the “Study Guides” button on the course Canvas site. Tests include 30 multiple choice and 20 true/false questions (50 questions, 2 points each

= 100 points). Students are allotted the class period (1 hour & 15 minutes) to take the test.

 

2)Interpersonal Relationship short essay/ Discussion Question vis a vi Modern Love: 25points

Using theories of interpersonal communication, students will analyze scenes/ in the film When Harry Met Sally; or, students may choose to answer essay questions provided by the instructor.

 

3)Am I being human enough : 25 points

A 4- to 6-minute extemporaneous (conversational; with notes) speech on an informative topic selected by the student and approved by the instructor. ImportantNot attending class on the scheduled day of a speech will result in a grade of “F” for the speech. In the case of an extreme emergency, the student must contact the instructor via phone or email.

 

4) Group Presentation: WHMS & TWM A 15- to 20-minute 180 points

Creative small group presentation or chapter presentation designed to inform your audience. The group topic is selected through “brainstorming” by the class. Important: Not attending class on the scheduled day of a group presentation will result in a grade of “F” for the presentation. In the case of an extreme emergency the student must phone/email/text both the instructor and his/her group members.

 

 

 

 

Grading

In order to pass this class, students must complete all of the assignments listed above. Failure to complete any one of these assignments will result in a semester grade of “F.” If an emergency situation results in a missed assignment, the student should immediately contact the instructor to discuss the situation. Possible Points: . Total of 529 points may be adjusted. New assignment may be introduced during the semester, thus altering the point total for the class. Your final numerical grade is calculated by adding the total points earned this semester divided by the total possible points. For example, if you earned 460 total points, you would divide those points 604 possible points. 460/529= 86 According to the chart below an 86% is a B

 

A= 90- 100

B= 80- 89

C= 70- 79

D= 60-69

F= 59 and below

 

Classroom Expectations

0. Students are expected to behave as adults. Rude, disrespectful behavior to either the instructor or other students, in either the classroom or online, will result in removal from the class with a grade of “F.”

0. I do understand that mental health plays an important role in your ability to do and completed assignments but it can’t be used as an alibi to discard assignments altogether. If you are struggling with mental health issues, it is important that you inform me. Already, there is a generous due date extension of 7 days, for any assignment regardless of your condition. If you need more time, I need to know.

0. All students are expected to check their NOVA email frequently and to be familiar with the course Canvas site. Students can activate their official NOVA email account at https://nvcc.my.vccs.edu/jsp/userconfig.jsp?action=reset .

0. The use of laptops for taking notes is allowed only if the student submits appropriate documentation indicating special needs. The use of flat electronic tablets (i.e. Ipad) is allowed; however, the student must first discuss the matter with the professor and also sit in the front of the classroom. Phones must be put away during class.

0. Students may turn in late assignments ONLY up until a week past the original due date. For example is an assignment is due 1/15/ 2024 a student has up until 1/22/2024to complete the assignment.

 

 

 

 

Attendance Policy:

 

Attendance and participation are central to student success in this communication class.

It is important that you understand the following:

1. National accreditation standards require student attendance. The most reasonable excuses will not change the fact that when you are absent you miss not only the content, but also the experience of the class. If you miss more than 25% of class sessions you will not be able to pass the course and will receive an automatic “F,” regardless of the number of points you have accumulated.

 

1. There may be an occasion where you must miss class and can receive an “excused absence” from me. These would include work, military or medical situations that are explained by official documentation. A court date due to legal violations will not receive an excused absence.

 

1. It is expected that you will be on time for class and will stay the entire class period.

 

 

1. If you find yourself in a position where you might miss significant amounts of class time, or will be late often due to extenuating circumstances, you should consult with me as soon as possible so that we can discuss the situation.

 

 

 

From Office of Emergency Management- Safety Insert-MA CampusMap-Trailside AssemblyAreas-Student

https://learn.vccs.edu/courses/422772/files/117266756/download?download_frd=1

 

Webcam

https://learn.vccs.edu/courses/422772/files/128131891/download?download_frd=1

 

Zoom

https://learn.vccs.edu/courses/422772/files/117977194/download?download_frd=1

 

 

 

INTRODUCTORY STATEMENT Students are responsible for knowing and following the policies in the Student Handbook. The following are highlights of information that students should be aware of as they begin a course. Academic Integrity Policy Accommodations and Accessibility Services Buy Books with Excess Financial Aid Career Services Closing Information Communication Course Drop/Withdrawal Policy Financial Stability and Advocacy Centers Forms to Request Services and Assistance Accommodations and Accessibility Services: Request for Alternate Format Course grade appeal Enrollment Services: Course Repeat Request Enrollment Services: Enrollment with Permission Financial Aid: Satisfactory Academic Progress (SAP) Appeal Form Parking Services: Vehicle Registration Form Student Grievance Forms Veteran Enrollment Request Form (VERF) Important Dates Spring 2024 Important Dates Spring 2024 Breaks & Holidays Libraries Office of Wellness and Mental Health Prerequisite Verification Statement Remote Student Support Services Student Rights and Responsibilities Student Integrity and Conduct Student Ombuds Services (SOS) TITLE IX Tutoring Spring 2024 2 Academic Integrity Policy NOVA promotes and emphasizes the importance of honesty in academic work. It is therefore imperative for students to maintain the highest standard of honor in their scholastic work. Academic dishonesty, as outlined in more detail in the Academic Integrity Policy (Policy Number: 224), can include, but is not limited to cheating on an exam or quiz, submitting work that is not your own (plagiarism), or sharing assessments online. Consequences of academic dishonesty can include a failing grade on an assignment, a failing grade in the course, and may include additional administrative sanctions such as suspension or expulsion from the college. Procedures for disciplinary measures and appeals are outlined in the Academic Integrity Procedures. It is a student’s responsibility to become familiar with the student code of conduct. Lack of awareness is no excuse for noncompliance with NOVA’s policies and procedures. NOVA Faculty may choose to use tools such as Turnitin to detect and flag instances of plagiarism in academic writing. These tools can also identify text written by artificial intelligence (AI) applications such as ChatGPT and flag it as potential plagiarism. The use of AI to complete assigned work may be considered a violation of the academic integrity policy. Students are urged to be cautious when using any web tools designed to assist with assignments without their instructors’ explicit permission to do so and to cite their sources in all their work. Accommodations and Accessibility Services NOVA is committed to ensuring all students have an opportunity to pursue a college education regardless of the presence or absence of a disability. No academically qualified student with a disability will be denied access to or participation in the services, programs, and activities of the College. Your access to and inclusion in this course is important to NOVA and me. Please request your accommodation letter (Memorandum of Accommodations) early in the semester or as soon as you become registered so that we have adequate time to arrange your approved academic accommodations. Returning students must renew their Memorandum of Accommodations (MOA) every semester; these students should submit the request 24 hours or later after enrolling in at least one class. Allow up to 7 business days for the request to be approved. Accommodations are provided for in-person, online, and remote/synchronous (Zoom) learning. To get started, review NOVA’s Accommodation and Accessibility Services website. Following a meeting with a counselor, you will be issued a Memorandum of Accommodation (MOA). You must provide your MOA to your professors, testing proctor, and/or tutoring center in order to receive your accommodations. You may provide your MOA any time during the semester; however, accommodations are not retroactive. You may email your MOA or provide me with a printed copy. I will send you an email to acknowledge receipt. If I have any questions or if there is anything about your accommodations you wish to explain, we will schedule a meeting outside of class for that purpose. Please remind me of any special arrangements that must be made in advance of tests and assessments. If you need a sign language interpreter, or if you need live captions for your Zoom class, send an email to interpreters@nvcc.edu. Buy Books with Excess Financial Aid If your anticipated financial aid is more than what you owe for the semester, you may use your excess aid to purchase books, supplies, or even a laptop through the NOVA Bookstore. Book purchases must occur during the provided disbursement dates every semester. Note that this excess financial aid can only be used to purchase course books (physical and digital) and supplies at the Spring 2024 3 NOVA Bookstore, not from other vendors. Two disbursement periods are available during the fall and spring semesters. Any holds on a student’s account regarding a debt to the College must be resolved prior to using financial aid at the bookstore. Career Services The College is committed to providing career services to all students as part of the comprehensive educational journey. Career Services assists students with exploring, developing and setting goals related to each student’s unique educational and academic needs. These services include career assessments, occupational information, goal setting, planning and employment resources. You can request an appointment with a career counselor. Closing Information NOVA announces campus and college closings on the NOVA homepage. You can also receive notification by cell phone or email if you register for NOVA Alert. Also review NOVA’s guidance on emergency closings, delayed openings, and continuation of instruction. If a course is canceled due to a weather event or other unforeseen situation, check the course Canvas site or NOVA email (@email.vccs.edu) as soon as possible for instructions and assignments to avoid falling behind in coursework. You are expected to be up to date with all assignments the next time the class meets. Communication Northern Virginia Community College (NVCC) faculty, staff, and administrators communicate with students through their official NVCC email accounts (________@nvcc.edu). Students are likewise required to use their VCCS email accounts (__________@email.vccs.edu) to communicate with instructors and other college personnel. Students should check their email accounts regularly. Course Drop/Withdrawal Policy Please note these important deadlines related to your enrollment in a course: ● Students may drop courses through NOVAConnect until the last day to drop with a tuition refund (census date). Students who drop a class during this period will receive a full refund. ● Requests to change your grade status to audit must also be completed before the last day to drop with a tuition refund (census date). ● Students who do not attend at least one class meeting or participate in an online learning class by the last day to drop with a tuition refund (census date) may be administratively withdrawn from the class. This means that there will be no record of the class or any letter grade on the student’s transcript. The student’s tuition will not be refunded. ● The Last Day to Withdraw is the last day to withdraw from classes without a grade penalty. Students will receive a grade of W. Students may withdraw from a course through NOVAConnect. The student’s tuition will not be refunded. Withdrawing from a course after the census date and before the withdrawal date will result in a “W” grade appearing on your transcript. To identify these important dates for your courses, please visit the College Academic Spring 2024 4 Calendar and scroll down to the specific session for your course. Please note that any drops or withdrawals from a course may impact financial aid, international student status, or military benefits. Students with questions should check with the appropriate offices. Financial Stability and Advocacy Centers The Financial Stability and Advocacy Centers provide assistance to students who are experiencing financial hardships that might prevent the students’ academic success. The personnel at the Financial Stability and Advocacy Centers work with students to identify college or community services available. For more information, please visit the Financial Stability and Advocacy Centers webpage, or contact the office by calling 703.323.3450 or emailing financialstability@nvcc.edu. Forms to Request Services and Assistance NOVA’s Forms Library is where you can access forms frequently used by the College. The forms are available in Portable Document Format (.pdf), Online or Excel. Some of the forms you will find online include: Accommodations and Accessibility Services: Request for Alternate Format Course grade appeal Enrollment Services: Course Repeat Request Enrollment Services: Enrollment with Permission Financial Aid: Satisfactory Academic Progress (SAP) Appeal Form Parking Services: Vehicle Registration Form Student Grievance Forms Veteran Enrollment Request Form (VERF) You also have the option to Make a Report to the college. Here is the list of reports you can submit: Report an academic violation. Report a student conduct violation (non-academy integrity). Report Sexual Misconduct (Title IX). Report a student needed assistance (non-conduct). Title IX Pregnant and Parenting Accommodation Request. Ombuds Assistance Request. Important Dates The academic calendar provides information about important dates and deadlines each semester. Visit this calendar to learn about Open registration dates. Academic advising dates. Registration dates. Deadlines to pay tuition fees. First and Last day of classes Spring 2024 5 Deadline to drop the class with a tuition refund or change classes to audit. Deadline to withdraw without grade penalty (W) and no refund. Final Exams Schedule. Breaks and holidays. Financial aid disbursement dates. Deadline to apply for graduation. Spring 2024 Important Dates Description 15 Week Term 1st 7 Week Term 13 Week Term 2nd 7 Week Term Classes Begin January 16 January 16 January 30 March 19 Students May Add or Drop Courses Through NOVAConnect January 16-22 January 16-18 January 30- Februay 4 March 19-21 Last day to drop with a tuition refund or change to audit (census date)* February 1 January 23 February 13 March 26 Last day to withdraw without grade penalty** March 23 February 14 March 28 April 17 Last Day of Final Exams Final Exam Schedule May 6 March 4 May 6 May 6 Spring 2024 Breaks & Holidays Martin Luther King, Jr. Holiday observed for students, faculty and staff. College offices closed. January 15 Spring Break March 11-17 * To find the last day to drop with tuition refund (census date) for a variable duration (dynamic) course, go to schedule of class, search for the course, then click on the class number. ** Withdrawals after this date require documentation of mitigating circumstances and permission of the instructor and the academic dean. Please check NOVAConnect for start dates and census dates for variable duration (dynamic) sessions. Libraries NOVA Libraries support the mission of the College to deliver in-person and online postsecondary teaching, learning, and workforce development to students, faculty, staff and community members. The libraries promote equity and inclusion by addressing systemic barriers to student success and fostering intellectual curiosity, creativity and critical thinking. At our libraries, you may borrow books/printed material, access article databases, access videos, connect to Wi-Fi, use the printers, and reserve a study room. Many of our academic resources may be accessed remotely as well. Spring 2024 6 Office of Wellness and Mental Health During your time at NOVA, you may experience challenges including struggles with academics, finances, or your personal well-being. NOVA has support resources available. Please contact the Office of Wellness and Mental Health if you are seeking resources and support, or if you are worried about a friend or classmate. Prerequisite Verification Statement As noted in the Course Prerequisites Policy, some courses have prerequisite or corequisite requirements that are established to foster a student’s success in the course. Students may not enroll in a course for which they do not meet the prerequisites by the time the course begins or for which they do not simultaneously enroll in any corequisite. Students may be administratively dropped from any course for which they have not met the prerequisite. If a course has a prerequisite, it is the responsibility of the student to ensure completion of this pre-requisite course first. Any student needing assistance in determining prerequisite or corequisite requirements can reach out to their faculty member or Campus Academic Division office for support. Remote Student Support Services If you need academic assistance or need college services but cannot make it to campus, many of our services can be accessed remotely. We can provide assistance from several departments, including the following: Academic Advising Canvas and Course Resources (guides and tutorials) Financial Aid IT Help Desk Library Support Student Rights and Responsibilities The Office of Student Rights and Responsibilities (OSRR) promotes and encourages honesty, integrity, and respect among NOVA students and staff. We accomplish our mission through education, compliance through behavioral standards, and support of individual rights. Our work helps enhance the quality of the NOVA community and equips students with the knowledge, skills, and resources to be successful inside and outside of the classroom. The Office of Student Rights and Responsibilities (OSRR) comprise the following areas: Student Integrity and Conduct Investigate alleged violations of the Student Code of Conduct. Adjudication of informal and formal Code of Conduct hearings Mediation (settling differences) Student development workshops Professional development workshop on topics like Conflict Resolution and Classroom Management Referrals to resources and services Student Ombuds Services (SOS) For students who believe an unfair resolution was made for a formal complaint made to NVCC (i.e., academic concerns, ethical behavior, violation of College policies and/or procedures, business services, financial aid, etc.), SOS will investigate the student’s concern. Confidential – students may discuss problems or issues in a confidential manner Independent – of any individual/office within NVCC Spring 2024 7 Informal – review matters received and make informal inquiries to offer resolution options, make referrals, and mediate disputes. Neutral – strives for impartiality, fairness, and objectivity in the treatment of people and the consideration of issues Voluntary – it is the student’s responsibility to contact SOS for services Contact: Dr. Takesha McMiller Interim Associate Vice President, Student Support Services Phone: 703.323.3768 Email: tamcmiller@nvcc.edu TITLE IX Title IX is a civil rights law that prohibits discrimination on the basis of sex in educational programs, activities, admission, and employment. Complaints of sex-based discrimination, sexual violence, domestic violence, dating violence, and sexual or gender-based harassment are governed by the Title IX Policy. For more information or to make a report, visit the Office of Title IX. Tutoring NOVA’s Tutoring Centers offer free in-person and virtual tutoring to all NOVA Nighthawks. Students can drop by any one of campus Tutoring Centers for walk-in services or set up an appointment for tutoring. To request an appointment for one-onone tutoring, either in-person or via Zoom, log in to myNOVA to select EAB Navigate. For more information and for Tutoring Center locations, visit the NOVA Tutoring Services page. In addition, Tutor.com is an online tutoring service that Northern Virginia Community College offers that is free to all students. Tutor.com provides tutoring in a variety of subjects, many of which are available 24/7. To access Tutor.com, click on the Tutor.com: 24/7 Online Tutoring link located in your course on the navigation menu.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

(subject to change)

 

ALL READINGS SHOULD BE READ AND READY TO DISCUSS ON THE DUE DATE.

Week/Date

F Friday

 

Chapter Content/Activities Chapters Covered Assignments Due
F Jan 19 Course Overview

Peer Interviews/Introductions

 

  Waking Life; Language & Comm. 15 points

Attendance Base Only

F Jan 26 The Basics of Communication Chap. 1 1 Minute Racist Blog. 15 points

Attendance Base Only

 

F Feb. 2

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Perception & Self

*Communication, Culture & Diversity:

 

 

 

 

 

Chapter 3 will be discussed within the context of perception and the self.

Chapt’s. 2& 3 The Onion 7 pts

 

 

 

 

 

Asynchronous portion- complete at home:

Johari Window 20 points

(You are creating your Window)

F- Feb 9 Watch Film: Tuesday With Morrie

 

 

 

Chap. 4 Discussion: Am I Being Human Enough Check-In 10 points.
F- Feb 16 Dialogical Communication Continued. Chap 4 Buber Exercise7 points

 

 

F- Feb. 23 Work On Group Projects TWM

 

 

  Progress Report Must Be Turned In. 5 points

Discussion: Am I Being Human Enough Check-In 10 points.

 

       
F. Mar 1

 

 

  Chap. 6 TWM Presentations 90pts
F. Mar 8

 

 

F. Mar 22

 

 

 

 

 

 

F. Mar 29

 

 

 

Relationship Development Maintenance

Chapt’s 8 & 9 Exam I: Chapters 1-7

 

 

Ted Talk Exercise & NYT 15 pts/

 

Modern Love article 25 pts

 

 

Dialogical Skit. Preparation

7.5 pts

 

 

 

F – Apr 5 Chpt 8 Relationship Development &

 

Chapt9. Relationship Maintenance Communication in intimate relationships.

Ch. 10 Dialogical Skit Due 7.5pts

 

 

 

Film Viewing WHMS

 

Fri Apr 12 Am I Being Human Enough?   Discussion: Am I Being Human Enough Check-In 10 points.
Fri. Apr 19 WHMS Reaction Paper or Presentation   WHMS: Reaction Paper or Presentations.
Fri. Apr. 26- May 3 Student/ Professor Meeting &

Am I Being Human Enough Project

 

 

  Am I Being Human Enough Project Due. 30 pts.
 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology – Lab Assignment

Psychology – Lab Assignment

10/27/2016 Week 2 (Oct 24 ­ Oct 30) – PSYC­330­OL50­16FA1

https://blackboard.campbell.edu/webapps/blackboard/content/listContent.jsp?content_id=_2095381_1&course_id=_36869_1&nolaunch_after_review=true&outco… 1/2

Lab Activity 2 PART A: Identifying experimental variables

Identify the independent and dependent variables in the following descriptions of experiments:

1. Students watched a cartoon either alone or with others and then rated how funny they found the cartoon to be.

2. A comprehension test was given to students after they had studied textbook material either in silence or with the television turned on.

3. Some elementary school teachers were told that a child’s parents were college graduates, and other teachers were told that the child’s parents had not finished high school; they then rated the child’s academic potential.

4. Workers at a company were assigned to one of two conditions: one group completed a stress management­training program; another group of workers did not participate in the training. The number of sick days taken by these workers was examined for the two subsequent months

PART B: Nonexperimental observations

Respond to the following questions:

1. You observe that classmates who get good grades tend to sit toward the front of the classroom, while those who receive poor grades tend to sit toward the back. What are three possible cause­ and­effect relationships for this nonexperimental observation?

2. A few years ago, newspapers reported a finding that Americans who have a glass of wine a day are healthier than those who have no wine (or who have a lot of wine or other alcohol). What are some plausible alternative explanations for this finding; that is, what variables other than wine could explain the finding?

3. The limitations of nonexperimental research were dramatically brought to the attention of the public by the results of an experiment on the effects of postmenopausal hormone replacement therapy (part of a larger study known as the Women’s Health Initiative). An experiment is called a clinical trial in medical research. In the clinical trial, participants were randomly assigned to receive either the hormone replacement therapy or a placebo (no hormones). The hormone replacement therapy consisted of estrogen plus progestin. In 2002, the investigators concluded that women taking the hormone replacement therapy had a higher incidence of heart disease than did women in the placebo (no hormone) condition. At that point, they stopped the experiment and informed both the participants and the public that they should talk with their physicians about the advisability of this therapy. The finding dramatically contrasted with the results of nonexperimental research in which women taking hormones had a lower incidence of heart disease; in these studies, researchers compared women who were already taking the hormones with women not taking hormones. Why do you think the results were different with the experimental research and the nonexperimental research? Explain.

PART C: Is it a correlation or is it an experiment?

For the following questions, first answer whether the study described is a correlation or is it in experiment. If it is a correlation, is it positive or negative? If it is an experiment, what are the independent and dependent variables?

1. Dr. Snoop and Dr. Dre are interested in the effects of smoking marijuana on learning, so they place several ferrets in a box where if they press a lever they will get a food pellet. Some ferrets are exposed to pot smoke 3 times a day; some for 1 time a day, and some not at all.  After two weeks of exposure they measure which groups of ferrets learn to push the lever faster.  They find that less pot smoke is associated with faster learning.

2. Researchers are interested in the effects of bystanders on altruistic (helping) behaviors, they have someone pretend to have a seizure when either several people are present or only one person and then see if helping behaviors are affected.  They find that people are more likely to help with fewer bystanders.

3. Researchers are interested in the effects of patterns of TV watching on children’s aggressive behavior.  They have kids keep a diary of what they are watching and for how long and then

 

 

10/27/2016 Week 2 (Oct 24 ­ Oct 30) – PSYC­330­OL50­16FA1

https://blackboard.campbell.edu/webapps/blackboard/content/listContent.jsp?content_id=_2095381_1&course_id=_36869_1&nolaunch_after_review=true&outco… 2/2

compare it to school reports of aggressive actions.  They find that the more aggressive TV a child watches the more aggressive schools report they are.

4. Dr. Mnemonic is interested in the effect of type of questioning on memory. She has several subjects watch a video of car accidents and then asks some of the subjects leading questions like, “was the car red?”. Othersare asked open­ended questions like “what color was the car?”. She then looks at how people remember events based on question type.

PART D: Studying behavior

In a study published by Golub, Gilbert, and Wilson (2009), two experiments and a field study were conducted in an effort to determine whether this negative expectation is a good thing or a bad thing. In the two laboratory studies, participants were asked to complete a personality assessment and were then led to have either positive, negative, or no expectations about the results. Participants’ affective (emotional) state was assessed prior to—and directly after— hearing a negative (in the case of study 1a) or positive (in the case of study 1b) outcome. In the field study, participants were undergraduate introductory psychology students who were asked about their expectations of their performance in an upcoming exam. Then, a day after the exam, positive and negative emotion was assessed. Taken together, the results of these three studies suggest that anticipating bad outcomes may be an ineffective path to positive emotion.

First, acquire and read the following article (note: you will need to obtain the full­text article via a Wiggins Memorial Library database search): Golub, S. A., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). Anticipating one’s troubles: The costs and benefits of negative expectations. Emotion, 9, 227– 281. doi:10.1037/a0014716 ­ Then, after reading the article, respond to the following: ­ 1. For each of the studies, how did Golub, Gilbert, and Wilson (2009) operationally define the positive expectations? How did they operationally define affect? 2. In experiments 1a and 1b, what were the independent variable(s)? What where the dependent variable(s)? 3. This article includes three different studies. In this case, what are the advantages to answering the research question using multiple methods? 4. On what basis did the authors conclude, “our studies suggest that the affective benefits of negative expectations may be more elusive than their costs” (p. 280)? 5. Evaluate the external validity of the two experiments and one field study that Golub, Gilbert, and Wilson (2009) conducted. 6. How good was the internal validity? Discuss and explain your reasoning.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology Quiz homework help

Psychology Quiz homework help

A client reports the following: “I like spaghetti. Worms dig in the earth. It might rain today.” This is an example of:

a. a delusion.

b. a hallucination.

c. a loosening of associations.

d. Avolition. 5 points

 

Question 2    Children with ____________ disorder demonstrate deficits in social interactions and communication and display repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and activities.

a. autistic

b. Rett’s

c. childhood disintegrative

d. autism spectrum 5 points

 

Question 3    Children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are more likely than children without a psychological disturbance to belong to families:

a. that strictly enforce discipline through physical punishment.

b. in which both parents are employed.

c. that experience frequent disruptions and aggressive parents.

d. from lower socioeconomic groups. 5 points

 

Question 4    Disorders characterized by impairment in two key areas of development including deficits in social interactions, communication with others, and repetitive patterns of behavior are called _____.   a

. chronic developmental disorders

b. autism spectrum disorder

c. intellectual development disorder

d. conduct disorder 5 points    Q

 

uestion 5    Individuals with ______ violate the rights of others and society’s norms or laws.

a. ADHD

b. autism spectrum disorder

c. conduct disorder

d. oppositional defiant disorder 5 points

 

Question 6    Kara has borderline personality disorder. She has a tendency to go from friendly and loving when she perceives someone as “good” to flying into a rage when she thinks that person has rejected her. Which of the following terms does Kara’s behavior exemplify?

a. Parasuicide.

b. Semantic dementia.

c. Psychopathy.

d. Splitting. 5 points

 

Question 7    Psychosis refers to knowing the difference between:

 

a. right and wrong.

b. conscious and unconscious desires.

c. what is real and what is not real.

d. pleasure and pain. 5 points

 

Question 8    Restricted affect refers to:

a. a loss of motivation to complete tasks.

b. a lack of pleasure.

c. an absence of emotional expression.

d. demonstration of delusions and hallucinations. 5 points

 

Question 9    Roger curses at his parents and calls his mother degrading names. He slapped his schoolteacher because she told him that he needed “to get to work” and he doesn’t like anyone telling him what to do. After being suspended from school, Roger’s parents put him on restriction and he was not to watch TV or leave the house; however, as soon as he got up in the morning, Roger would leave to go hang out at the local convenience store and smoke. Which of the following behaviors best characterizes Roger’s behavior?

a. Autism spectrum disorder.

b. ADHD.

c. Oppositional defiant disorder.

d. Developmental coordination disorder. 5 points

 

Question 10    The odd and eccentric behaviors seen in schizophrenia can resemble the symptoms of _____ disorder.

a. obsessive-compulsive

b. autism spectrum

c. histrionic personality

d. somatic symptom 5 points

 

Question 11    The personality disorder that is characterized by rigidness, perfectionism, and emotional blockage is:

a. avoidant personality disorder.

b. dependent personality disorder.

c. obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.

d. histrionic personality disorder. 5 points

 

Question 12    The term “psychopath” is synonymous with _____ personality disorder.

a. borderline

b. obsessive-compulsive

c. antisocial

d. narcissistic 5 points

 

Question 13    Vic has demonstrated a long-standing pattern of disreputable and manipulative behaviors. He has a drug abuse problem and a long criminal record for a variety of crimes. What is worse, he shows no remorse for some of the harsh things he has done. Vic would most likely be diagnosed as having _____ personality disorder.

a. borderline

b. histrionic

c. antisocial

d. avoidant 5 points

 

Question 14    Which of the following personality disorders is more likely to endorse beliefs such as “I don’t have to be bound by rules that apply to other people”?

a. Borderline personality disorder.

b. Schizotypal personality disorder.

c. Avoidant personality disorder.

d. Narcissistic personality disorder. 5 points

 

Question 15    Which of the following statements about children with autism spectrum disorder is FALSE?

a. They tend to become distressed when routines are changed.

b. They often develop narrow and specifically focused interests.

c. They may seem impervious to pain, heat, or cold.

d. They rarely notice changes in sound, light, or smell. 5 points

 

Question 16    Which of the following symptoms is characteristic of borderline personality disorder?

a. Hallucinations.

b. Manic behavior.

c. Major depressive episodes.

d. Unstable relationships. 5 points

 

Question 17    Which personality disorder is characterized by an unstable or disorganized identity and is accompanied with odd beliefs, perceptions, and self-presentations?

a. Paranoid personality disorder.

b. Borderline personality disorder.

c. Schizotypal personality disorder.

d. Dependent personality disorder. 5 points

 

Question 18    Which type of hallucinations is the most common in people with schizophrenia?

a. Auditory.

b. Visual.

c. Tactile.

d. Gustatory. 5 points

 

Question 19    Why is ADHD considered to be a controversial diagnosis?

a. There is no clinical data available to prove that the disorder exists.

b. Most clients diagnosed with ADHD are just drug-seeking patients claiming to have ADHD to get their physician just to prescribe them the stimulant medications to treat it.

c. Many mental health providers believe that the disorder is over diagnosed and is misapplied to individuals who are unwilling or unmotivated to focus on difficult or unpleasant tasks.

d. Because those diagnosed with the disorder are primarily unwilling or unmotivated to focus on difficult or unpleasant tasks and this label excuses this behavior. 5 points

 

Question 20    _____ are ideas that an individual believes are true but are highly unlikely and often simply impossible.   a

. Hallucinations

b. Psychoses

c. Deceptions

d. Delusions 5 points     Save and Submit

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Psychology homework help

Unit5DiscPeerResp2QDA Psychology homework help

Response Guidelines

Provide a substantive contribution that advances the discussion in a meaningful way by identifying strengths of the posting, challenging assumptions, and asking clarifying questions. Your response is expected to reference the assigned readings, as well as other theoretical, empirical, or professional literature to support your views and writings. Reference your sources using standard APA guidelines. Review the Participation Guidelines section of the Discussion Participation Scoring Guide to gain an understanding of what is required in a substantive response.

Peer 1 Response: Cait

Anderson & Bushman (2001) conducted the meta-analysis, Effects of Violent Video Games On Aggressive Behavior, Aggressive Cognition, Aggressive Affect, Physiological Arousal, and Prosocial Behavior: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Scientific Literature, which determined that there was a correlation between playing video games and aggressive behaviors. In a set of 21 controlled experimental studies, Anderson & Bushman (2001) concluded that there was a correlation between playing video games (x) and engaging in the aggressive behavior (y). Table 1 determined that aggressive behavior was measured at r= .19, therefore concluding that the correlation was statistically significant due to a large number of participants that were involved in the research study. Aggressive behavior in conjunction with playing violent video games was tested with 3,033 participants. If there was a smaller sample size (in this case, less than 3,033 participants), the correlation r=.19 may not have been as large or as significant. This result also yielded significant results because r=.19 is positive, rather than negative. In Table 1 it can also be determined that the results displayed significant results because of the homogeneity test. The homogeneity test determined an outcome of x2(32)  23.25, p > .05 (Anderson & Bushman, 2001). It was measured that the p score was 23.25, compared to the normal value used by SPSS and researchers, which is .05. Because the p-value was larger than .05, it yielded significant results for this research study. If the p-value was smaller than .05, it would not hold the same level of significance.

Reference

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12(5), 353–359.

Peer 2 Response: Teddick

A meta-analysis (Anderson & Bushman, 2001) reported that the average correlation between time spent playing video games (X) and engaging in aggressive behavior (Y) in a set of 21 well-controlled experimental studies was .19. This correlation was judged to be statistically significant. In your own words, what can you say about the nature of the relationship?

Warner (2013) explained that in a result of .19 will fall among the small (r<.10) and the medium (r<.30) and in less than the large (r<.50). This is an indicator that there is a compelling association, but this does not mean there is a causation between aggressive behavior and video games. Because of the insufficient information provided, we must consider every factor that contributes to the research, for example age of the gamer, time spent playing, time spent watching movies of violence, how much the parents are involved in their child’s life, if that person is involved with the wrong group of individuals, their surrounding neighborhood, and even their social and academic intellect. However, there still is a relationship of correlation on the meta-analysis, but does not necessarily means a causation. The meta-analysis shows a significant association on aggressive behavior being affected by playing violent video games, but does not prove or show that it causes the behavior. If anything it does encourage parents to look for more age appropriate video games and limit the access of violent-themed games for their kids (Anderson, C. & Bushman, B., 2001).

References:

Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2001). Effects of violent video games on aggressive behavior, aggressive cognition, aggressive affect, physiological arousal, and prosocial behavior: A meta-analytic review of the scientific literature. Psychological Science, 12(5), 353–359.

Warner, R. M. (2013). Applied Statistics: From Bivariate Through Multivariate Techniques (2nd ed.). Sage Publications.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Selection of a Statistical Analysis Approach homework help

Selection of a Statistical Analysis Approach homework help

Discussion 1: Selection of a Statistical Analysis Approach

Though data analysis occurs after the study has completed a data collection stage, the researcher needs to have in mind what type of analysis will allow the researcher to obtain an answer to a research question. The researcher must understand the purpose of each method of analysis, the characteristics that must be present in the study for the design to be appropriate and any weaknesses of the design that might limit the usefulness of the study results. Only then can the researcher select the appropriate design. Choosing the appropriate design enables the researcher to claim the data that is potential evidence that provides information about the relationship being studied. Notice that it is not the statistical test which tells us that research is valid, rather, it is the research design. Social workers must be aware of and adjust any limitations of their chosen design that may impact the validity of the study.

To prepare for this Discussion, review the handout, A Short Course in Statistics and pages 210–220 in your course text Social Work Evaluation: Enhancing What We Do. If necessary, locate and review online resources concerning internal validity and threats to internal validity. Then, review the “Social Work Research: Chi Square” case study located in this week’s resources. Consider the confounding variables, that is, factors that might explain the difference between those in the program and those waiting to enter the program.

· Post an interpretation of the case study’s conclusion that “the vocational rehabilitation intervention program may be effective at promoting full-time employment.”

· Describe the factors limiting the internal validity of this study, and explain why those factors limit the ability to draw conclusions regarding cause and effect relationships.

References (use 3 or more)

Dudley, J. R. (2014). Social work evaluation: Enhancing what we do.(2nd ed.) Chicago, IL: Lyceum Books.

  • Chapter 9, “Is the Intervention Effective?” (pp. 226–236: Read from “Determining a Causal Relationship” to “Outcome Evaluations for Practice”)

Document:Stocks, J. T. (2010). Statistics for social workers. In B. Thyer (Ed.), The handbook of social work research methods(2nd ed., pp. 75–118). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. (PDF)

Trochim, W. M. K. (2006). Internal validity. Retrieved from http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/intval.php

Document:Week 4: A Short Course in Statistics Handout (PDF)

Document:Week 4: Handout: Chi-Square findings (PDF)

Discussion 2: Looking Through Different Lenses

As a social worker, you bring your own lens—that is, your own set of assumptions, biases, beliefs, and interpretations—into your interactions with clients and the human services professionals with whom you collaborate. Human services organizations have their own cultures that influence their organizational lenses. An organizational lens reflects key assumptions about the individuals to whom the organization provides services. These assumptions influence the organization’s policies and procedures which, in turn, impact service delivery. For example, an organization that focuses on understanding the perspectives of the clients it serves may allow clients to provide feedback about their client experience through membership on advisory boards or boards of directors. The clients may have the power to make recommendations and decisions about the organization’s policies and procedures.

Understanding cultural lenses—your personal lens, as well as those of the organizations and other individuals with whom you work and interact—will enable you to better serve your clients.

Focus on the Paula Cortez case study for this Discussion. In this case study, four professionals present their perspectives on the Paula Cortez case. These workers could view Paula’s case through a variety of cultural lenses, including socioeconomic, gender, ethnicity, and mental health. For this Discussion, you take the role of the social worker on the case and interpret Paula’s case using two of these lenses.

·  Post how you, as a social worker, might interpret the needs of Paula Cortez, the client, through the two cultural lenses you selected.

·  Then, explain how, in general, you would incorporate multiple perspectives of a variety of stakeholders and/or human services professionals as you treat clients.

References (use 3 or more)

Northouse, P. G. (2013). Leadership: Theory and practice (6th ed.). Los Angeles: Sage Publications

Chapter 15, “Culture and Leadership” (pp. 383–421)

Chun-Chung Chow, J., & Austin, M. J. (2008). The culturally responsive social service agency: The application of an evolving definition to a case study. Administration in Social Work, 32(4), 39–64.

Laureate Education (Producer). (2014a). Cortez case study [Multimedia]. Retrieved from https://class.waldenu.edu

Cortez Family: A Meeting of an Interdisciplinary Team

Paula has just been involuntarily hospitalized and placed on the psychiatric unit, for a minimum of 72 hours, for observation. Paula was deemed a suicidal risk after an assessment was completed by the social worker. The social worker observed that Paula appeared to be rapidly decompensating, potentially placing herself and her pregnancy at risk.

Paula just recently announced to the social worker that she is pregnant. She has been unsure whether she wanted to continue the pregnancy or terminate. Paula also told the social worker she is fearful of the father of the baby, and she is convinced he will try to hurt her. He has started to harass, stalk, and threaten her at all hours of the day. Paula began to exhibit increased paranoia and reported she started smoking again to calm her nerves. She also stated she stopped taking her psychiatric medications and has been skipping some of her HIV medications.

The following is an interdisciplinary team meeting being held in a conference room at the hospital. Several members of Paula’s team (HIV doctor, psychiatrist, social worker, and OB nurse) have gathered to discuss the precipitating factors to this hospitalization. The intent is to craft a plan of action to address Paula’s noncompliance with her medications, increased paranoia, and the pregnancy.

Physician 

Dialogue 1

Paula is a complicated patient, and she presents with a complicated situation. She is HIV positive, has Hepatitis C, and multiple foot ulcers that can be debilitating at times. Paula has always been inconsistent with her HIV meds—no matter how often I explain the need for consistent compliance in order to maintain her health. Paula has exhibited a lack of insight into her medical conditions and the need to follow instructions. Frankly, I was astonished and frustrated when she stopped her wound care treatments and started to use chamomile tea on her foot ulcers. Even though we have educated her to the negative consequences of stopping her meds, and trying alternative medications instead, she continues to do so.

Psychiatrist 

Dialogue 1

As Paula’s psychiatrist for close to 10 years, I have followed her progress in and out of the hospital for quite a while—and I know her very well. She is often non-compliant with her medications, randomly stopping them after she reports she doesn’t like the way they make her feel. She has been hospitalized to stabilize her medications several times over the last 10 years, although she has managed to stay out of the psychiatric unit for the last three. Recently, she had seemed to appreciate the benefits of taking her medications and her compliance has much improved. She had been seeing her social worker regularly, and her overall mental health and physical health were improving. This has changed recently, after several stressful life events. We learned that Paula was pregnant by a man she met briefly at a local flower shop. She also reports he has been harassing her with threatening phone calls and unwarranted visits to her home. Paula disclosed to the social worker that she was neither eating nor taking her medication—and she had not gotten out of bed for days. Her decompensation was rapid and extremely worrisome and, therefore, called for a 72-hour hold.

OB Nurse

Dialogue 1

I have not known the patient long, but it does appear that she is trying her best to deal with a very difficult situation. Pregnancies are stressful times for even the healthiest of women. For Paula to learn she is pregnant at 43—in addition to her HIV and Hepatitis status and her bipolar diagnosis—must be so overwhelming. Adding to this, she has come to her two appointments alone and stated she has no one to bring along with her. When I inquired about the father of the child, she said he’s a bad man and he won’t leave her alone. She seemed truly frightened of him and appears convinced he will hurt her.

Social Worker

Dialogue 1

When Paula came to me and told me she was pregnant, I was indeed shocked by this announcement. She had never mentioned dating anyone, and with her multiple medical and psychiatric issues, I never thought this would be an issue we would address. Paula and I have developed a strong working relationship over the last two years, and she has shared many private emotions and thoughts. This relationship has been tested, though, since I suggested she be admitted to the hospital. Paula was furious with me, accusing me of locking her up and not helping her. It will take time to repair our working relationship. Once I rebuild that rapport, we will need to work together to find a way to address all of her concerns. We will need a plan that will address her medical needs, her psychiatric needs, and the needs of her unborn child.

Physician

Dialogue 2

As far as her pregnancy, if Paula doesn’t take her HAART medications religiously, she risks having a baby who is HIV positive. I am concerned about how she is going to care for a baby with her multiple medical issues. On the practical side, I wonder how she will physically care for this child. She has a semi-paralyzed right hand and walks with a limp. Additionally, when her foot ulcers flare up, she can barely put pressure on her feet. Newborns take a lot of time and energy, and I am not sure she has the capacity to handle the needs of an infant—let alone a toddler. I have not made any formal recommendations to Paula regarding whether to continue the pregnancy, but I have told Paula that, if she does decide to have the child, she must take her HAART medications every day. I explained that this is vital to her health and the health of her unborn child.

Psychiatrist

Dialogue 2

When her social worker, who I am in regular contact with, informed me that Paula announced she was pregnant, I was obviously concerned. Knowing Paula as well as I do, I felt I could be honest with her and give her my opinion about the situation. I told her that she should abort. Based on her medical history, including her physical and mental health disabilities, I did not believe she had the capacity to care for this unborn child. She has absolutely no support at all, outside of the treatment team, and would have no familial assistance to take care of this child. My recommendation for abortion was only solidified when we had to involuntarily hospitalize her. I fear that Paula cannot take care of herself, and she cannot be trusted to take her medications. If she does decide to continue with the pregnancy, my recommendation would be that she stay on the psychiatric unit for her entire pregnancy. That way, we will know that she is taking her medications and that the fetus is safe.

OB Nurse

Dialogue 2

Paula is most definitely a high-risk pregnancy, but that does not mean she can’t have a healthy baby. If she keeps up with her HAART medications and comes to her prenatal visits, there’s no reason this baby can’t be born healthy and HIV negative. My larger concern is with the pain medications she takes for her foot ulcers. There is a slight chance the baby will be born addicted to them. We would have to plan for a stay in the NICU if that occurs. While Paula clearly started to decompensate and exhibited some very risky behaviors recently, I think we should try and understand the stress she has been under. While it is not my place to tell the patient what she should do about a pregnancy, I don’t see that we would have to recommend termination.

Social Worker

Dialogue 2

Paula has overcome many obstacles in her life, but a baby—at her age and with her medical profile—is very different. Paula has made many bad decisions in her life, and the decision to keep this baby may or may not be the best for both her and the child. That being said, if her decision is to continue the pregnancy, we need to find a way to face the mountain of obstacles. She has little to no social support, and there will be many difficulties she will face caring for the baby alone. Paula also has limited financial resources and will need to apply for WIC and Medicaid. There are the numerous supplies that we will need to obtain, such as a crib, clothing, diapers, and formula. She has historically been unreliable about following up with referrals, so she is going to need a lot of encouragement and support. Honestly, I may not believe this pregnancy is a good idea, although I would never tell her that—that’s not up to me or anyone else. We all, ultimately, need to accept her decision and move on. Our goal now is to help Paula make it safely through this pregnancy and work on a plan to help her care for this baby once it is born. I don’t agree that she should be kept on the psychiatric unit for the next seven or eight months. Allowing Paula to play an active role in preparing for the baby is an important task, and she will need to be out in the community and in her home taking care of things. We have to show that we believe in her and her willingness to manage this situation to the best of her ability. We need to affirm her strengths and support her weaknesses.

Discussion 3:  Administration and Culturally Competent Advocacy

Social work administrators can use their roles as leaders to increase cultural competency within their organizations and, thus, help to create positive social change. As social work administrators critically assess situations in which social injustice or inequality has taken place, they may discover an organizational need for increased cultural competency. However, changing the culture of an organization is not an easy task since administrators must address personal and organizational assumptions about diversity and cultural competency simultaneously.

For this Discussion, consider how social work administrators might apply their leadership roles to increase cultural competency within their organizations.

· Post at least two strategies social workers may use to become advocates for social change through cultural competence.

· In addition, identify at least two challenges administrators may face in developing cultural competency within their organizations.

References (use 3 or more)

Northouse, P. G. (2013). Leadership: Theory and practice(6th ed.). Los Angeles: Sage Publications

Reprinted by permission of Sage Publications via the Copyright Clearance Center.

Chapter 15, “Culture and Leadership” (pp. 383–421)

Chun-Chung Chow, J., & Austin, M. J. (2008). The culturally responsive social service agency: The application of an evolving definition to a case study. Administration in Social Work, 32(4), 39–64.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

The Importance Of Random Assignment

The Importance Of Random Assignment

Short Paper 5-1

 

The Importance of Random Assignment

PSY-510-X1150 Research Methods in Psych

 

 

Random Selection and Random Assignment; two terms that should go synonymous with research study and crucial components to all things statistics. A firm grasp on these two concepts can be the difference between observing true results and just documenting an observation without real substance. An understanding of the difference between random selection and random assignment is critical to learning the benefits they provide and it should be explained before discussing their uses and contributions to a study.

Random selection is a method for obtaining a researcher’s sample group to be used in their study. The sample group is a manageable selection of subjects or participants in a study that should represent a much larger population. Random assignment is equally important to any study and is very similar to random selection, but differs in the sense that it decides how to utilize a sample population instead of how to collect it. There are many situations, in which only a random selection or only random assignment might be utilized and some even where neither play roles (Trochim, 2006). It is often found that the best or most accurate results are found when both are employed together, which is made clear by stating the benefits of random selection and random assignment.

For any researcher it is a challenge for their study group to be as much a clone of the larger population of focus as possible. For example, a study of how many people in America that love monster trucks might not be a good representation of the general American public if only white males are questioned, which account for only about 36% of the U.S. populous according to the 2001 U.S. census (U.S Census, 2001). This is where random sampling can be utilized. The more random or haphazard the sample group is generated, the most representative of the host population it usually is. Using the same example, it would be more accurate to question people at the mall because there is a much more diverse presence consisting of multiple races, beliefs, and gender. However, there is a possibility that even this method of collection may not be as random as it appears and could be improved. If the mall selected is in a specific demographic area that a large percentage of mall-goers are of a specific gender or race etc. then the study would only show how that group feels about monster trucks. To improve this, more malls should be included, but even then care must be taken that the collectors or questioners are asking anyone who will give an answer or asking every nth person entering the mall to remove their own personal bias. It is simple enough to think of ways to gather random samples and give insight to the benefits of the method but it is not the only tool at a researcher’s disposal to represent a large population in a study.

Random assignment ensures that in a study where there is a control group and a test group that the test or control group is not limited to only one subcategory of test participants. This means that a study of The Mozart Effect or the study of Mozart’s music as described by Frances Rauscher in 1993 would not represent most people if the control or the test group ended up consisting of only people who greatly dislike classical music (Jenkins, 2001). To prevent this detriment to the study the random selection technique is applied; except now it is applied to the sample group instead of the total population. All members of the sample population will be divided into one category or another instead of just reducing the size of the study group so that results can be compared and contrasted. With understanding of how these methods can be utilized it is easy to see how they benefit, but they do not always have to work in tandem.

An example that can still be an effective study that is nearly impossible to use random selection is a study where the sample group is made up of people that choose for themselves if they will participate or not. A study of this nature may consist of a sample group of a specific age group, gender, cultural group, etc. that is interested in the results of the study; therefore skewing the results. On the other hand there can be studies that begin with a large random sample, but specific people are chosen to be a control group to root out a unique occurrence. Although the two generalizing tools are not be used at the same time they still have the same benefits by themselves as they did when being used together. The generalizing tools of random selection and random assignment both increase the accuracy of nearly any research situation or study with minimal effort, but it is important to recognize that there always exist outside influences that can offset the benefits of random selection and assignment, such as a person feeling uncharacteristic of themselves during the study. There are other methods to combat these outside influences, but random selection and random assignment are simple steps towards meaningful observations that will illuminate several very common detrimental influences.

 

Resources:

Jenkins, J. S. (2001). The Mozart effect. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 94, 170-172. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1281386/?tool=pubmed#ref1

U.S. Census Bureau. (2001, September 10). Census 2000 Summary File 1. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/population/cen2000/phc-t11/tab01.pdf

Trochim, W. (2006). Random Selection & Assignment. Retrieved from http://www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/random.php

 

1

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Basic Concepts And Principles Of Psychology.

Basic Concepts And Principles Of Psychology.

CHAPTER 1: Thinking Critically With Psychological Science

What Is Psychology?

A smile is a smile the world around

Throughout this book, you will see examples not only of our cultural and gender diversity but also of the similarities that define our shared human nature. People in different cultures vary in when and how often they smile, but a naturally happy smile means the same thing anywhere in the world.

For people whose exposure to psychology comes from popular books, magazines, TV, and the Internet, psychologists seem to analyze personality, offer counseling, and dispense child-rearing advice. Do they? Yes, and much more. Consider some of psychology’s questions, which perhaps have also been yours:

·  Have you ever found yourself reacting to something as one of your biological parents would—perhaps in a way you vowed you never would—and then wondered how much of your personality you inherited? To what extent do genes predispose our person-to-person personality differences? To what extent do home and community environments shape us?

·  Have you ever worried about how to act among people of a different culture, race, gender, or sexual orientation? In what ways are we alike as members of the human family? How do we differ?

·  Have you ever awakened from a nightmare and, with a wave of relief, wondered why you had such a crazy dream? How often, and why, do we dream?

·  Have you ever played peekaboo with a 6-month-old and wondered why the baby finds the game so delightful? The infant reacts as though, when you momentarily move behind a door, you actually disappear—only to reappear out of thin air. What do babies actually perceive and think?

·  Have you ever wondered what fosters school and work success? Are some people just born smarter? And does sheer intelligence explain why some people get richer, think more creatively, or relate more sensitively?

·  Have you ever wondered how the Internet, video games, and electronic social networks affect people? How do today’s electronic media influence how we think and how we relate?

·  Have you ever become depressed or anxious and wondered whether you’ll ever feel “normal”? What triggers our bad moods—and our good ones? What’s the line between a normal mood swing and a psychological disorder for which someone should seek help?

Psychology is a science that seeks to answer such questions.

Psychology’s Roots

1-1: What are some important milestones in psychology’s development?

To assist your active learning of psychology, Learning Objectives, framed as questions, appear at the beginning of major sections. You can test your understanding by trying to answer the question before, and then again after, you read the section.

To be human is to be curious about ourselves and the world around us. Before 300 B.C.E., the Greek naturalist and philosopher Aristotle theorized about learning and memory, motivation and emotion, perception and personality. Today we chuckle at some of his guesses, like his suggestion that the source of our personality is the heart. But credit Aristotle with asking the right questions.

Psychological Science Is Born

Information sources are cited in parentheses, with name and date. Every citation can be found in the end-of-book References, with complete documentation that follows American Psychological Association style.

Philosophers’ thinking about thinking continued until the birth of psychology on a December day in 1879, in a small, third-floor room at Germany’s University of Leipzig. There, two young men were helping an austere, middle-aged professor, Wilhelm Wundt, create an experimental apparatus. Their machine measured the time lag between people’s hearing a ball hit a platform and their pressing a telegraph key (Hunt, 1993). Curiously, people responded in about one-tenth of a second when asked to press the key as soon as the sound occurred—and in about two-tenths of a second when asked to press the key as soon as they were consciously aware of perceiving the sound. (To be aware of one’s awareness takes a little longer.) Wundt was seeking to measure “atoms of the mind”—the fastest and simplest mental processes. So began the first psychological laboratory, staffed by Wundt and by psychology’s first graduate students.

Before long, this new science of psychology became organized into different branches, or schools of thought, each promoted by pioneering thinkers. Two early schools were structuralism and functionalism. As physicists and chemists discerned the structure of matter, so Wundt’s student Edward Bradford Titchener aimed to discover the mind’s structure. He engaged people in self-reflective introspection (looking inward), training them to report elements of their experience as they looked at a rose, listened to a metronome, smelled a scent, or tasted a substance. What were their immediate sensations, their images, their feelings? And how did these relate to one another? Alas, introspection proved somewhat unreliable. It required smart, verbal people, and its results varied from person to person and experience to experience. As introspection waned, so did structuralism.

structuralism early school of thought promoted by Wundt and Titchener; used introspection to reveal the structure of the human mind.

functionalism early school of thought promoted by James and influenced by Darwin; explored how mental and behavioral processes function—how they enable the organism to adapt, survive, and flourish.

Throughout the text, important concepts are boldfaced. As you study, click on the terms for the definitions. You can also find these terms with their definitions in the Terms and Concepts to Remember section of the ebook.

Hoping to assemble the mind’s structure from simple elements was rather like trying to understand a car by examining its disconnected parts. Philosopher-psychologist William James thought it would be more fruitful to consider the evolved functions of our thoughts and feelings. Smelling is what the nose does; thinking is what the brain does. But why do the nose and brain do these things? Under the influence of evolutionary theorist Charles Darwin, James assumed that thinking, like smelling, developed because it was adaptive—it contributed to our ancestors’ survival. Consciousness serves a function. It enables us to consider our past, adjust to our present, and plan our future. As a functionalist, James encouraged explorations of down-to-earth emotions, memories, willpower, habits, and moment-to-moment streams of consciousness.

As these names illustrate, the early pioneers of most fields, including psychology, were predominantly men. In 1890, over the objections of Harvard’s president, James admitted Mary Whiton Calkins into his graduate seminar (Scarborough & Furumoto, 1987). (In those years women lacked even the right to vote.) When Calkins joined, the other students (all men) dropped out. So James tutored her alone. Later, she finished all of Harvard’s Ph.D. requirements, outscoring all the male students on the qualifying exams. Alas, Harvard denied her the degree she had earned, offering her instead a degree from Radcliffe College, its undergraduate “sister” school for women. Calkins resisted the unequal treatment and refused the degree. She nevertheless went on to become a distinguished memory researcher and the American Psychological Association’s (APA’s) first female president in 1905.

The honor of being the first female psychology Ph.D. later fell to Margaret Floy Washburn, who also wrote an influential book, The Animal Mind, and became the APA’s second female president in 1921.

Study Tip: Memory research reveals a testing effect: We retain information much better if we actively retrieve it by self-testing and rehearsing. To bolster your learning and memory, take advantage of the Retrieve It opportunities you’ll find throughout this text.

 

 

 

Psychological Science Develops

In the field’s early days, many psychologists shared with the English essayist C. S. Lewis the view that “there is one thing, and only one in the whole universe which we know more about than we could learn from external observation.” That one thing, Lewis said, is ourselves: “We have, so to speak, inside information” (1960, pp. 18–19). Wundt and Titchener focused on inner sensations, images, and feelings. James engaged in introspective examination of the stream of consciousness and emotion. For these and other early pioneers, psychology was defined as “the science of mental life.”

And so it continued until the 1920s, when the first of two provocative American psychologists appeared on the scene. John B. Watson, and later B. F. Skinner, dismissed introspection and redefined psychology as “the scientific study of observable behavior.” You cannot observe a sensation, a feeling, or a thought, they said, but you can observe and record people’s behavior as they respond to different situations. Many agreed, and the behaviorists became one of psychology’s two major forces well into the 1960s.

behaviorism the view that psychology (1) should be an objective science that (2) studies behavior without reference to mental processes. Most research psychologists today agree with (1) but not with (2).

The other major force was Freudian psychology, which emphasized the ways our unconscious thought processes and our emotional responses to childhood experiences affect our behavior. (In chapters to come, we’ll look more closely at Sigmund Freud’s ideas.)

As the behaviorists had rejected the early 1900s definition of psychology, two other groups rejected the behaviorist definition in the 1960s. The first, the humanistic psychologists, led by Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow, found both Freudian psychology and behaviorism too limiting. Rather than focusing on the meaning of early childhood memories or on the learning of conditioned responses, the humanistic psychologists drew attention to ways that current environmental influences can nurture or limit our growth potential, and the importance of having our needs for love and acceptance satisfied.

humanistic psychology historically significant perspective that emphasized the growth potential of healthy people.

The second group of psychologists pioneered the 1960s cognitive revolution, leading the field back to its early interest in mental processes. Cognitive psychology scientifically explores how we perceive, process, and remember information, and even why we can get anxious or depressed. Cognitive neuroscience, an interdisciplinary study, has enriched our understanding of the brain activity underlying mental activity.

cognitive neuroscience the interdisciplinary study of the brain activity linked with cognition (including perception, thinking, memory, and language).

To encompass psychology’s concern with observable behavior and with inner thoughts and feelings, today we define psychology as the science of behavior and mental processes. Let’s unpack this definition. Behavior is anything an organism does—any action we can observe and record. Yelling, smiling, blinking, sweating, talking, and questionnaire marking are all observable behaviors. Mental processes are the internal, subjective experiences we infer from behavior—sensations, perceptions, dreams, thoughts, beliefs, and feelings.

psychology the science of behavior and mental processes.

The key word in psychology’s definition is science. Psychology is less a set of findings than a way of asking and answering questions. My aim, then, is not merely to report results but also to show you how psychologists play their game. You will see how researchers evaluate conflicting opinions and ideas. And you will learn how all of us, whether scientists or simply curious people, can think smarter when describing and explaining the events of our lives.

 

Contemporary Psychology

This young science of psychology developed from the more established fields of philosophy and biology. Wundt was both a philosopher and a physiologist. Ivan Pavlov, who pioneered the study of learning (Chapter 7), was a Russian physiologist. Freud was an Austrian physician. Jean Piaget, the last century’s most influential observer of children, (Chapter 4), was a Swiss biologist. James was an American philosopher. This list of pioneering psychologists—“Magellans of the mind,” as Morton Hunt (1993) has called them—illustrates psychology’s origins in many disciplines and countries.

Like the pioneers, today’s psychologists are citizens of many lands. The International Union of Psychological Science has 71 member nations, from Albania to Zimbabwe. Psychology is growingand it is globalizing. The story of psychology is being written in many places, with interests ranging from nerve cell activity to international conflicts.

Psychology’s Biggest Question

1-2: What is psychology’s historic big issue?

Are our human traits present at birth, or do they develop through experience? The debate over this huge nature–nurture issue is ancient. The Greek philosopher Plato (428–348 B.C.E.) assumed that we inherit character and intelligence and that certain ideas are inborn. Aristotle (384–322 B.C.E.) countered that there is nothing in the mind that does not first come in from the external world through the senses.

nature-nurture issue the longstanding controversy over the relative contributions that genes and experience make to the development of psychological traits and behaviors. Today’s psychological science sees traits and behaviors arising from the interaction of nature and nurture.

More insight into nature’s influence on behavior arose after a 22-year-old seafaring voyager, Charles Darwin, pondered the incredible species variation he encountered, including tortoises on one island that differed from those on nearby islands. His 1859 On the Origin of Species explained this diversity by proposing the evolutionary process of natural selection: From among chance variations, nature selects traits that best enable an organism to survive and reproduce in a particular environment. Darwin’s principle of natural selection is still with us 150+ years later as biology’s organizing principle, and now an important principle for twenty-first-century psychology. This would surely have pleased Darwin, for he believed his theory explained not only animal structures (such as a polar bear’s white coat) but also animal behaviors (such as the emotional expressions associated with human lust and rage).

natural selection the principle that, among the range of inherited trait variations, those contributing to reproduction and survival will most likely be passed on to succeeding generations.

A nature-made nature–nurture experiment

The nature–nurture issue recurs throughout this text as today’s psychologists explore the relative contributions of biology and experience, asking, for example, how we humans are alike (because of our common biology and evolutionary history) and diverse (because of our differing environments). Are gender differences biologically predisposed or socially constructed? Is children’s grammar mostly innate or formed by experience? How are intelligence and personality differences influenced by heredity, and by environment? Are sexual behaviors more “pushed” by inner biology or “pulled” by external incentives? Should we treat psychological disorders—depression, for example—as disorders of the brain, disorders of thought, or both?

Over and over again we will see that in contemporary science the nature–nurture tension dissolves: Nurture works on what nature endows. Our species is biologically endowed with an enormous capacity to learn and adapt. Moreover, every psychological event (every thought, every emotion) is simultaneously a biological event. Thus, depression can be both a brain disorder and a thought disorder.

Psychology’s Three Main Levels of Analysis

1-3: What are psychology’s levels of analysis and related perspectives?

Each of us is a complex system that is part of a larger social system. But each of us is also composed of smaller systems, such as our nervous system and body organs, which are composed of still smaller systems—cells, molecules, and atoms.

These tiered systems suggest different levels of analysis, which offer complementary outlooks. It’s like explaining why grizzly bears hibernate. Is it because hibernation helped their ancestors to survive and reproduce? Because their inner physiology drives them to do so? Because cold environments hinder food gathering during winter? Such perspectives are complementary because “everything is related to everything else” (Brewer, 1996). Together, different levels of analysis form a biopsychosocial approach, which integrates biological, psychological, and social-cultural factors

levels of analysis the differing complementary views, from biological to psychological to social-cultural, for analyzing any given phenomenon.

biopsychosocial approach an integrated approach that incorporates biological, psychological, and social-cultural levels of analysis.

 

Each level provides a vantage point for viewing a behavior or mental process, yet each by itself is incomplete. Like different academic disciplines, psychology’s varied perspectives ask different questions and have their own limits. The different perspectives described in  TABLE 1.1 complement one another. Consider, for example, how they shed light on anger:

·  Someone working from a neuroscience perspective might study brain circuits that cause us to be “red in the face” and “hot under the collar.”

·  Someone working from the evolutionary perspective might analyze how anger facilitated the survival of our ancestors’ genes.

·  Someone working from the behavior genetics perspective might study how heredity and experience influence our individual differences in temperament.

·  Someone working from the psychodynamic perspective might view an outburst as an outlet for unconscious hostility.

·  Someone working from the behavioral perspective might attempt to determine which external stimuli trigger angry responses or aggressive acts.

·  Someone working from the cognitive perspective might study how our interpretation of a situation affects our anger and how our anger affects our thinking.

·  Someone working from the social-cultural perspective might explore how expressions of anger vary across cultural contexts.

The point to remember: Like two-dimensional views of a three-dimensional object, each of psychology’s perspectives is helpful. But each by itself fails to reveal the whole picture.

 

 

 

Table 1.1: Psychology’s Current Perspectives

Perspective Focus Sample Questions
Neuroscience How the body and brain enable emotions, memories, and sensory experiences How do pain messages travel from the hand to the brain? How is blood chemistry linked with moods and motives?
Evolutionary How the natural selection of traits has promoted the survival of genes How does evolution influence behavior tendencies?
Behavior genetics How our genes and our environment influence our individual differences To what extent are psychological traits such as intelligence, personality, sexual orientation,and vulnerability to depression products of our genes? Of our environment?
Psychodynamic How behavior springs from unconscious drives and conflicts How can someone’s personality traits and disorders be explained by unfulfilled wishes and childhood traumas?
Behavioral How we learn observable responses How do we learn to fear particular objects or situations? What is the most effective way to alter our behavior, say, to lose weight or stop smoking?
Cognitive How we encode, process, store, and retrieve information How do we use information in remembering? Reasoning? Solving problems?
Social-cultural How behavior and thinking vary across situations and cultures How are we alike as members of one human family? How do we differ as products of our environment?

 

Psychology’s Subfields

1-4: What are psychology’s main subfields?

Picturing a chemist at work, you probably envision a white-coated scientist surrounded by glassware and high-tech equipment. Picture a psychologist at work and you would be right to envision

·  a white-coated scientist probing a rat’s brain.

·  an intelligence researcher measuring how quickly an infant shows boredom by looking away from a familiar picture.

·  an executive evaluating a new “healthy life styles” training program for employees.

·  someone at a computer analyzing data on whether adopted teens’ temperaments more closely resemble those of their adoptive parents or their biological parents.

·  a therapist listening carefully to a client’s depressed thoughts.

·  a traveler visiting another culture and collecting data on variations in human values and behaviors.

·  a teacher or writer sharing the joy of psychology with others.

 

Psychology in court

The cluster of subfields we call psychology is a meeting ground for different disciplines. Thus, it’s a perfect home for those with wide-ranging interests. In its diverse activities, from biological experimentation to cultural comparisons, psychology is united by a common quest: describing and explaining behavior and the mind underlying it.

Some psychologists conduct basic research that builds psychology’s knowledge base. In the pages that follow we will meet a wide variety of such researchers, including biological psychologistsexploring the links between brain and mind; developmental psychologists studying our changing abilities from womb to tomb; cognitive psychologists experimenting with how we perceive, think, and solve problems; personality psychologists investigating our persistent traits; and social psychologists exploring how we view and affect one another.

basic research pure science that aims to increase the scientific knowledge base.

These and other psychologists also may conduct applied research, tackling practical problems. Industrial-organizational psychologists, for example, use psychology’s concepts and methods in the workplace to help organizations and companies select and train employees, boost morale and productivity, design products, and implement systems.

applied research scientific study that aims to solve practical problems.

Although most psychology textbooks focus on psychological science, psychology is also a helping profession devoted to such practical issues as how to have a happy marriage, how to overcome anxiety or depression, and how to raise thriving children. As a science, psychology at its best bases such interventions on evidence of effectivenessCounseling psychologists help people to cope with challenges and crises (including academic, vocational, and marital issues) and to improve their personal and social functioning. Clinical psychologists assess and treat mental, emotional, and behavior disorders. Both counseling and clinical psychologists administer and interpret tests, provide counseling and therapy, and sometimes conduct basic and applied research. By contrast, psychiatrists, who also may provide psychotherapy, are medical doctors licensed to prescribe drugs and otherwise treat physical causes of psychological disorders.

counseling psychology a branch of psychology that assists people with problems in living (often related to school, work, or relationships) and in achieving greater well-being.

clinical psychology a branch of psychology that studies, assesses, and treats people with psychological disorders.

psychiatry a branch of medicine dealing with psychological disorders; practiced by physicians who sometimes provide medical (for example, drug) treatments as well as psychological therapy.

To balance historic psychology’s focus on human problems, Martin Seligman and others (2002, 2005, 2011) have called for more research on human strengths and human flourishing. Their positive psychology scientifically explores “positive emotions, positive character traits, and enabling institutions.” What, they ask, can psychology contribute to a “good life” that engages one’s skills, and to a “meaningful life” that points beyond oneself?

positive psychology the scientific study of human functioning, with the goals of discovering and promoting strengths and virtues that help individuals and communities to thrive.

With perspectives ranging from the biological to the social, and with settings from the laboratory to the clinic, psychology relates to many fields. Psychologists teach not only in psychology departments, but also in medical schools, law schools, and theological seminaries, and they work in hospitals, factories, and corporate offices. They engage in interdisciplinary studies, such as psychohistory (the psychological analysis of historical characters), psycholinguistics (the study of language and thinking), and psychoceramics (the study of crackpots). 1

Psychology: A science and a profession

Psychology also influences culture. And psychology deepens our appreciation for how we humans perceive, think, feel, and act. By so doing it can indeed enrich our lives and enlarge our vision. Through this book I hope to help guide you toward that end. As educator Charles Eliot said a century ago: “Books are the quietest and most constant of friends, and the most patient of teachers.”

 

The Need for Psychological Science

Although in some ways we outsmart the smartest computers, our intuition often goes awry. To err is human. Enter psychological science. With its procedures for gathering and sifting evidence, science restrains error. As we familiarize ourselves with its strategies and incorporate its underlying principles into our daily thinking, we can think smarter. Psychologists use the science of behavior and mental processes to better understand why people think, feel, and act as they do.

 

 

What About Intuition and Common Sense?

1-5: How do hindsight bias, overconfidence, and the tendency to perceive order in random events illustrate why science-based answers are more valid than those based on intuition and common sense?

The limits of intuition

Some people suppose that psychology merely documents and dresses in jargon what people already know: “So what else is new—you get paid for using fancy methods to prove what my grandmother knew?” Others place their faith in human intuition. Former President George W. Bush described the feeling to journalist Bob Woodward (2002) in explaining his decision to launch the Iraq war: “I’m a gut player. I rely on my instincts.” Today’s psychological science does document a vast intuitive mind. As we will see, our thinking, memory, and attitudes operate on two levels—conscious and unconscious—with the larger part operating off-screen, automatically. Like jumbo jets, we fly mostly on autopilot.

So, are we smart to listen to the whispers of our inner wisdom, to simply trust “the force within”? Or should we more often be subjecting our intuitive hunches to skeptical scrutiny?

This much seems certain: We often underestimate intuition’s perils. My geographical intuition tells me that Reno is east of Los Angeles, that Rome is south of New York, that Atlanta is east of Detroit. But I am wrong, wrong, and wrong. As novelist Madeleine L’Engle observed, “The naked intellect is an extraordinarily inaccurate instrument” (1973). Three phenomena—hindsight bias, judgmental overconfidence, and our tendency to perceive patterns in random events—illustrate why we cannot rely solely on intuition and common sense.

“Those who trust in their own wits are fools.”

Proverbs 28:26

Did We Know It All Along? Hindsight Bias

Consider how easy it is to draw the bull’s eye after the arrow strikes. After the stock market drops, people say it was “due for a correction.” After the football game, we credit the coach if a “gutsy play” wins the game, and fault the coach for the “stupid play” if it doesn’t. After a war or an election, its outcome usually seems obvious. Although history may therefore seem like a series of inevitable events, the actual future is seldom foreseen. No one’s diary recorded, “Today the Hundred Years War began.”

This hindsight bias (also known as the I-knew-it-all-along phenomenon) is easy to demonstrate: Give half the members of a group some purported psychological finding, and give the other half an opposite result. Tell the first group, “Psychologists have found that separation weakens romantic attraction. As the saying goes, ‘Out of sight, out of mind.’” Ask them to imagine why this might be true. Most people can, and nearly all will then view this true finding as unsurprising.

hindsight bias the tendency to believe, after learning an outcome, that one would have foreseen it. (Also known as the I-knew-it-all-along phenomenon.)

“Life is lived forwards, but understood backwards.”

Philosopher Søren Kierkegaard, 1813–1855

Tell the second group the opposite: “Psychologists have found that separation strengthens romantic attraction. As the saying goes, ‘Absence makes the heart grow fonder.’” People given this untrue result can also easily imagine it, and most will also see it as unsurprising. When two opposite findings both seem like common sense, there is a problem.

Such errors in our recollections and explanations show why we need psychological research. Just asking people how and why they felt or acted as they did can sometimes be misleading—not because common sense is usually wrong, but because common sense more easily describes what has happened than what will happen.

“Anything seems commonplace, once explained.”

Dr. Watson to Sherlock Holmes

Nevertheless, Grandma’s intuition is often right. As Yogi Berra once said, “You can observe a lot by watching.” (We have Berra to thank for other gems, such as “Nobody ever comes here—it’s too crowded,” and “If the people don’t want to come out to the ballpark, nobody’s gonna stop’em.”) Because we’re all behavior watchers, it would be surprising if many of psychology’s findings had not been foreseen. Many people believe that love breeds happiness, and they are right. Indeed, as Daniel Gilbert, Brett Pelham, and Douglas Krull (2003) have noted, “good ideas in psychology usually have an oddly familiar quality, and the moment we encounter them we feel certain that we once came close to thinking the same thing ourselves and simply failed to write it down.” Good ideas are like good inventions; once created, they seem obvious. (Why did it take so long for someone to invent suitcases on wheels and Post-it Notes?)

Hindsight bias

But sometimes Grandma’s intuition, informed by countless casual observations, has it wrong. In later chapters we will see how research has overturned popular ideas—that familiarity breeds contempt, that dreams predict the future, and that most of us use only 10 percent of our brain. We will also see how research has surprised us with discoveries about how the brain’s chemical messengers control our moods and memories, about other animals’ abilities, and about the effects of stress on our capacity to fight disease.

Overconfidence

We humans tend to think we know more than we do. Asked how sure we are of our answers to factual questions (Is Boston north or south of Paris?), we tend to be more confident than correct. 2 Or consider these three anagrams, which Richard Goranson (1978) asked people to unscramble:

WREAT → WATER

ETRYN → ENTRY

GRABE → BARGE

About how many seconds do you think it would have taken you to unscramble each of these? Knowing the answers tends to make us overconfident—surely the solution would take only 10 seconds or so? In reality, the average problem solver spends 3 minutes, as you also might, given a similar anagram without the solution: OCHSA. 3

Are we any better at predicting social behavior? Ohio State University psychologist Philip Tetlock (1998, 2005) collected more than 27,000 expert predictions of world events, such as the future of South Africa or whether Quebec would separate from Canada. His repeated finding: These predictions, which experts made with 80 percent confidence on average, were right less than 40 percent of the time. Nevertheless, even those who erred maintained their confidence by noting they were “almost right.” “The Québécois separatists almost won the secessionist referendum.”

Overconfidence in history:

“We don’t like their sound. Groups of guitars are on their way out.”

Decca Records, in turning down a recording contract with the Beatles in 1962

“Computers in the future may weigh no more than 1.5 tons.”

Popular Mechanics, 1949

“They couldn’t hit an elephant at this distance.”

General John Sedgwick, just before being killed during a U.S. Civil War battle, 1864

“The telephone may be appropriate for our American cousins, but not here, because we have an adequate supply of messenger boys.”

British expert group, evaluating the invention of the telephone

Perceiving Order in Random Events

In our natural eagerness to make sense of our world—what poet Wallace Stevens called our “rage for order”—we are prone to perceive patterns. People see a face on the moon, hear Satanic messages in music, or perceive the Virgin Mary’s image on a grilled cheese sandwich. Even in random data we often find order, because—here’s a curious fact of life—random sequences often don’t look random (Falk et al., 2009; Nickerson, 2002, 2005). In actual random sequences, patterns and streaks (such as repeating digits) occur more often than people expect (Oskarsson et al., 2009). To demonstrate this phenomenon for myself, I flipped a coin 51 times, with these results:

· 1. H

· 2. T

· 3. T

· 4. T

· 5. H

· 6. H

· 7. H

· 8. T

· 9. T

· 10. T

· 11. T

· 12. H

· 13. H

· 14. T

· 15. T

· 16. H

· 17. T

· 18. T

· 19. H

· 20. H

· 21. T

· 22. T

· 23. H

· 24. T

· 25. T

· 26. T

· 27. H

· 28. T

· 29. H

· 30. T

· 31. T

· 32. T

· 33. T

· 34. T

· 35. T

· 36. H

· 37. T

· 38. T

· 39. H

· 40. T

· 41. H

· 42. H

· 43. H

· 44. H

· 45. T

· 46. H

· 47. H

· 48. T

· 49. T

· 50. T

· 51. T

 

Looking over the sequence, patterns jump out: Tosses 10 to 22 provided an almost perfect pattern of pairs of tails followed by pairs of heads. On tosses 30 to 38 I had a “cold hand,” with only one head in nine tosses. But my fortunes immediately reversed with a “hot hand”—seven heads out of the next nine tosses. Similar streaks happen—about as often as one would expect in random sequences—in basketball shooting, baseball hitting, and mutual fund stock pickers’ selections (Gilovich et al., 1985; Malkiel, 2007; Myers, 2002). These sequences often don’t look random and so are over interpreted. (“When you’re hot, you’re hot!”)

What explains these streaky patterns? Was I exercising some sort of paranormal control over my coin? Did I snap out of my tails funk and get in a heads groove? No such explanations are needed, for these are the sorts of streaks found in any random data. Comparing each toss to the next, 23 of the 50 comparisons yielded a changed result—just the sort of near 50-50 result we expect from coin tossing. Despite seeming patterns, the outcome of one toss gives no clue to the outcome of the next.

“The really unusual day would be one where nothing unusual happens.”

Statistician Persi Diaconis (2002)

However, some happenings seem so extraordinary that we struggle to conceive an ordinary, chance-related explanation. In such cases, statisticians often are less mystified. When Evelyn Marie Adams won the New Jersey lottery twice, newspapers reported the odds of her feat as 1 in 17 trillion. Bizarre? Actually, 1 in 17 trillion are indeed the odds that a given person who buys a single ticket for each of two New Jersey lotteries will win both times. And given the millions of people who buy U.S. state lottery tickets, statisticians Stephen Samuels and George McCabe (1989) reported, it was “practically a sure thing” that someday, somewhere, someone would hit a state jackpot twice. Indeed, said fellow statisticians Persi Diaconis and Frederick Mosteller (1989), “with a large enough sample, any outrageous things are likely to happen.” An event that happens to but 1 in 1 billion people every day occurs about 7 times a day, over 2500 times a year.

The point to remember: Hindsight bias, overconfidence, and our tendency to perceive patterns in random events often lead us to overestimate our intuition. But scientific inquiry can help us sift reality from illusion.

Given enough random events, some weird-seeming streaks will occur

The Scientific Attitude: Curious, Skeptical, and Humble

1-6: How do the scientific attitude’s three main components relate to critical thinking?

Underlying all science is, first, a hard-headed curiosity, a passion to explore and understand without misleading or being misled. Some questions (Is there life after death?) are beyond science. Answering them in any way requires a leap of faith. With many other ideas (Can some people demonstrate ESP?), the proof is in the pudding. Let the facts speak for themselves.

 

The Amazing Randi

Magician James Randi has used this empirical approach when testing those claiming to see auras around people’s bodies:

RANDI: Do you see an aura around my head?

AURA SEER: Yes, indeed.

RANDI: Can you still see the aura if I put this magazine in front of my face?

AURA SEER: Of course.

RANDI: Then if I were to step behind a wall barely taller than I am, you could determine my location from the aura visible above my head, right?

Randi told me that no aura seer has agreed to take this simple test.

No matter how sensible-seeming or wild an idea, the smart thinker asks: Does it work? When put to the test, can its predictions be confirmed? Subjected to such scrutiny, crazy-sounding ideas sometimes find support. More often, science becomes society’s garbage disposal, sending crazy-sounding ideas to the waste heap, atop previous claims of perpetual motion machines, miracle cancer cures, and out-of-body travels into centuries past. To sift reality from fantasy, sense from nonsense, therefore requires a scientific attitude: being skeptical but not cynical, open but not gullible.

“I’m a skeptic not because I do not want to believe but because I want to know. I believe that the truth is out there. But how can we tell the difference between what we would like to be true and what is actually true? The answer is science.”

Michael Shermer, “I Want to Believe,”Scientific American, 2009

“To believe with certainty,” says a Polish proverb, “we must begin by doubting.” As scientists, psychologists approach the world of behavior with a curious skepticism, persistently asking two questions: What do you mean? How do you know?

Putting a scientific attitude into practice requires not only curiosity and skepticism but also humility—an awareness of our own vulnerability to error and an openness to surprises and new perspectives. In the last analysis, what matters is not my opinion or yours, but the truths nature reveals in response to our questioning. If people or other animals don’t behave as our ideas predict, then so much the worse for our ideas. This humble attitude was expressed in one of psychology’s early mottos: “The rat is always right.”

Historians of science tell us that these three attitudes—curiosity, skepticism, and humility—helped make modern science possible. Some deeply religious people today may view science, including psychological science, as a threat. Yet, many of the leaders of the scientific revolution, including Copernicus and Newton, were deeply religious people acting on the idea that “in order to love and honor God, it is necessary to fully appreciate the wonders of his handiwork” (Stark, 2003a,b).

“My deeply held belief is that if a god anything like the traditional sort exists, our curiosity and intelligence are provided by such a god. We would be unappreciative of those gifts…if we suppressed our passion to explore the universe and ourselves.”

Carl Sagan, Broca’s Brain, 1979

Of course, scientists, like anyone else, can have big egos and may cling to their preconceptions. Nevertheless, the ideal of curious, skeptical, humble scrutiny of competing ideas unifies psychologists as a community as they check and recheck one another’s findings and conclusions.

 

Critical Thinking

The scientific attitude prepares us to think smarter. Smart thinking, called critical thinking, examines assumptions, discerns hidden values, evaluates evidence, and assesses conclusions. Whether reading a news report or listening to a conversation, critical thinkers ask questions. Like scientists, they wonder, How do they know that? What is this person’s agenda? Is the conclusion based on anecdote and gut feelings, or on evidence? Does the evidence justify a cause-effect conclusion? What alternative explanations are possible?

critical thinking: thinking that does not blindly accept arguments and conclusions. Rather, it examines assumptions, discerns hidden values, evaluates evidence, and assesses conclusions.

Has psychology’s critical inquiry been open to surprising findings? The answer, as ensuing chapters illustrate, is plainly Yes. Believe it or not, massive losses of brain tissue early in life may have minimal long-term effects. Within days, newborns can recognize their mother’s odor and voice. After brain damage, a person may be able to learn new skills yet be unaware of such learning. Diverse groups—men and women, old and young, rich and middle class, those with disabilities and without—report roughly comparable levels of personal happiness.

And has critical inquiry convincingly debunked popular presumptions? The answer, as ensuing chapters also illustrate, is again Yes. The evidence indicates that sleepwalkers are not acting out their dreams. Our past experiences are not all recorded verbatim in our brains; with brain stimulation or hypnosis, one cannot simply “hit the replay button” and relive long-buried or repressed memories. Most people do not suffer from unrealistically low self-esteem, and high self-esteem is not all good. Opposites do not generally attract. In each of these instances and more, what psychological science has learned is not what is widely believed.

“The real purpose of the scientific method is to make sure Nature hasn’t misled you into thinking you know something you don’t actually know.”

Robert M. Pirsig, Zen and the Art of Motorcycle Maintenance, 1974

How Do Psychologists Ask and Answer Questions?

Psychologists arm their scientific attitude with the scientific method—a self-correcting process for evaluating ideas with observation and analysis. In its attempt to describe and explain human nature, psychological science welcomes hunches and plausible-sounding theories. And it puts them to the test. If a theory works—if the data support its predictions—so much the better for that theory. If the predictions fail, the theory will be revised or rejected.

The Scientific Method

1-7: How do theories advance psychological science?

In everyday conversation, we often use theory to mean “mere hunch.” In science, a theory explains with principles that organize observations and predict behaviors or events. By organizing isolated facts, a theory simplifies. By linking facts with deeper principles, a theory offers a useful summary. As we connect the observed dots, a coherent picture emerges.

theory an explanation using an integrated set of principles that organizes observations and predicts behaviors or events.

A good theory about sleep deprivation’s effects on memory, for example, helps us organize countless sleep-related observations into a short list of principles. Imagine that we observe over and over that people with good sleep habits tend to answer questions accurately in class, and they do well at test time. We might therefore theorize that sleep improves memory. So far so good: Our sleep-retention principle neatly summarizes a list of facts about the effects of sleep loss.

Yet no matter how reasonable a theory may sound—and it does seem reasonable to suggest that sleep loss could affect memory—we must put it to the test. A good theory produces testable predictions, called hypotheses. By enabling us to test and to reject or revise our theory, such predictions direct research. They specify what results would support the theory and what results would disconfirm it. To test our theory about the effects of sleep on memory, we might assess people’s retention of course materials after a good night’s sleep, or a shortened night’s sleep

hypothesis a testable prediction, often implied by a theory.

 

Our theories can bias our observations. Having theorized that better memory springs from more sleep, we may see what we expect: We may perceive sleepy people’s comments as less insightful. The urge to see what we expect is ever-present, both inside and outside the laboratory, as when people’s views of climate change influence their interpretation of local weather events.

As a check on their biases, psychologists report their research with precise operational definitionsof procedures and concepts. Hunger, for example, might be defined as “hours without eating,” generosity as “money contributed,” sleep loss as “hours less” than one’s natural sleep. Using these carefully worded statements, others can  replicate  (repeat) the original observations with different participants, materials, and circumstances. If they get similar results, confidence in the finding’s reliability grows. The first study of hindsight bias aroused psychologists’ curiosity. Now, after many successful replications with different people and questions, we feel sure of the phenomenon’s power.

operational definition a statement of the procedures (operations) used to define research variables. For example, human intelligence may be operationally defined as “what an intelligence test measures.”

replication repeating the essence of a research study, usually with different participants in different situations, to see whether the basic finding extends to other participants and circumstances.

In the end, our theory will be useful if it (1) organizes a range of self-reports and observations, and (2) implies predictions that anyone can use to check the theory or to derive practical applications. (Does people’s sleep predict their retention?) Eventually, our research may lead to a revised theory that better organizes and predicts what we know. Or, our research may be replicated and supported by similar findings.

As we will see next, we can test our hypotheses and refine our theories using descriptive methods (which describe behaviors, often through case studies, naturalistic observations, or surveys), correlational methods (which associate different factors), and experimental methods (which manipulate factors to discover their effects). To think critically about popular psychology claims, we need to understand these methods and know what conclusions they allow.

 

Description

1-8: How do psychologists use case studies, naturalistic observations, and surveys to observe and describe behavior, and why is random sampling important?

The starting point of any science is description. In everyday life, we all observe and describe people, often drawing conclusions about why they act as they do. Professional psychologists do much the same, though more objectively and systematically, through

·  case studies (in-depth analyses of special individuals).

·  naturalistic observation (watching and recording individuals’ behavior in their natural setting).

·  surveys and interviews (self-reports in which people answer questions about their behavior or attitudes).

“‘Well my dear,’ said Miss Marple, ‘human nature is very much the same everywhere, and of course, one has opportunities of observing it at closer quarters in a village.’”

Agatha Christie, The Tuesday Club Murders, 1933

 

The Case Study

Among the oldest research methods, the  case study  examines one individual in depth in the hope of revealing things true of us all. Some examples: Much of our early knowledge about the brain came from case studies of individuals who suffered a particular impairment after damage to a certain brain region. Jean Piaget taught us about children’s thinking after carefully observing and questioning only a few children. Studies of only a few chimpanzees revealed their capacity for understanding and language. Intensive case studies are sometimes very revealing. They show us what can happen, and they often suggest directions for further study.

case study an observation technique in which one person is studied in depth in the hope of revealing universal principles.

But individual cases may mislead us if the individual is atypical. Unrepresentative information can lead to mistaken conclusions. Indeed, anytime a researcher mentions a finding (“Smokers die younger: 95 percent of men over 85 are nonsmokers”) someone is sure to offer a contradictory anecdote (“Well, I have an uncle who smoked two packs a day and lived to be 89”). Dramatic stories and personal experiences (even psychological case examples) command our attention and are easily remembered. Journalists understand that, and so begin an article about bank foreclosures with the sad story of one family put out of their house, not with foreclosure statistics. Stories move us. But stories can mislead. Which of the following do you find more memorable? (1) “In one study of 1300 dream reports concerning a kidnapped child, only 5 percent correctly envisioned the child as dead” (Murray & Wheeler, 1937). (2) “I know a man who dreamed his sister was in a car accident, and two days later she died in a head-on collision!” Numbers can be numbing, but the plural of anecdote is not evidence. As psychologist Gordon Allport (1954, p. 9) said, “Given a thimbleful of [dramatic] facts we rush to make generalizations as large as a tub.”

The point to remember: Individual cases can suggest fruitful ideas. What’s true of all of us can be glimpsed in any one of us. But to discern the general truths that cover individual cases, we must answer questions with other research methods.

 

 

 

 

 

Naturalistic Observation

A second descriptive method records behavior in natural environments. These  naturalistic observations  range from watching chimpanzee societies in the jungle, to unobtrusively videotaping (and later systematically analyzing) parent-child interactions in different cultures, to recording racial differences in students’ self-seating patterns in a school lunchroom.

naturalistic observation observing and recording behavior in naturally occurring situations without trying to manipulate and control the situation.

Like the case study, naturalistic observation does not explain behavior. It describes it. Nevertheless, descriptions can be revealing. We once thought, for example, that only humans use tools. Then naturalistic observation revealed that chimpanzees sometimes insert a stick in a termite mound and withdraw it, eating the stick’s load of termites. Such unobtrusive naturalistic observations paved the way for later studies of animal thinking, language, and emotion, which further expanded our understanding of our fellow animals. “Observations, made in the natural habitat, helped to show that the societies and behavior of animals are far more complex than previously supposed,” chimpanzee observer Jane Goodall noted (1998). Thanks to researchers’ observations, we know that chimpanzees and baboons use deception. Psychologists Andrew Whiten and Richard Byrne (1988) repeatedly saw one young baboon pretending to have been attacked by another as a tactic to get its mother to drive the other baboon away from its food. The more developed a primate species’ brain, the more likely it is that the animals will display deceptive behaviors (Byrne & Corp, 2004).

A natural observer

Naturalistic observations also illuminate human behavior. Here are three findings you might enjoy:

·  A funny finding. We humans laugh 30 times more often in social situations than in solitary situations. (Have you noticed how seldom you laugh when alone?) As we laugh, 17 muscles contort our mouth and squeeze our eyes, and we emit a series of 75-millisecond vowel-like sounds, spaced about one-fifth of a second apart (Provine, 2001).

·  Sounding out students. What, really, are introductory psychology students saying and doing during their everyday lives? To find out, Matthias Mehl and James Pennebaker (2003) equipped 52 such students from the University of Texas with belt-worn Electronically Activated Recorders (EARs). For up to four days, the EAR captured 30 seconds of the student’s waking hours every 12.5 minutes, thus enabling the researchers to eavesdrop on more than 10,000 half-minute life slices by the end of the study. On what percentage of the slices do you suppose they found the students talking with someone? What percentage captured the students at a computer keyboard? The answers: 28 and 9 percent. (What percentage of your waking hours are spent in these activities?)

·  Culture, climate, and the pace of life. Naturalistic observation also enabled Robert Levine and Ara Norenzayan (1999) to compare the pace of life in 31 countries. (Their operational definition of pace of life included walking speed, the speed with which postal clerks completed a simple request, and the accuracy of public clocks.) Their conclusion: Life is fastest paced in Japan and Western Europe and slower paced in economically less-developed countries. People in colder climates also tend to live at a faster pace (and are more prone to die from heart disease).

Naturalistic observation offers interesting snapshots of everyday life, but it does so without controlling for all the factors that may influence behavior. It’s one thing to observe the pace of life in various places, but another to understand what makes some people walk faster than others.

An EAR for naturalistic observation

The Survey

 survey  looks at many cases in less depth. A survey asks people to report their behavior or opinions. Questions about everything from sexual practices to political opinions are put to the public. In recent surveys,

·  half of all Americans reported experiencing more happiness and enjoyment than worry and stress on the previous day (Gallup, 2010).

·  1 in 5 people across 22 countries reported believing that alien beings have come to Earth and now walk among us disguised as humans (Ipsos, 2010b).

·  68 percent of all humans—some 4.6 billion people—say that religion is important in their daily lives (Diener et al., 2011).

survey a technique for ascertaining the self-reported attitudes or behaviors of a particular group, usually by questioning a representative, random sample of the group.

But asking questions is tricky, and the answers often depend on question wording and respondent selection.

 

Wording Effects

Even subtle changes in the order or wording of questions can have major effects. People are much more approving of “aid to the needy” than of “welfare,” of “affirmative action” than of “preferential treatment,” of “not allowing” televised cigarette ads and pornography than of “censoring” them, and of “revenue enhancers” than of “taxes.” In 2009, three in four Americans in one national survey approved of giving people “a choice” of public (government-run) or private health insurance. Yet in another survey, most Americans were not in favor of “creating a public health care plan administered by the federal government that would compete directly with private health insurance companies” (Stein, 2009). Because wording is such a delicate matter, critical thinkers will reflect on how the phrasing of a question might affect people’s expressed opinions.

Random Sampling

In everyday thinking, we tend to generalize from cases we observe, especially vivid cases. Given (a) a statistical summary of a professor’s student evaluations and (b) the vivid comments of a biased sample—two irate students—an administrator’s impression of the professor may be influenced as much by the two unhappy students as by the many favorable evaluations in the statistical summary. The temptation to ignore the sampling bias and to generalize from a few vivid but unrepresentative cases is nearly irresistible.

So how do you obtain a representative sample—say, of the students at your college or university? It’s not always possible to survey the whole group you want to study and describe. How could you choose a group that would represent the total student population? Typically, you would seek a random sample, in which every person in the entire group has an equal chance of participating. You might number the names in the general student listing and then use a random number generator to pick your survey participants. (Sending each student a questionnaire wouldn’t work because the conscientious people who returned it would not be a random sample.) Large representative samples are better than small ones, but a small representative sample of 100 is better than an unrepresentative sample of 500.

population all those in a group being studied, from which samples may be drawn. (Note: Except for national studies, this does not refer to a country’s whole population.)

random sample a sample that fairly represents a population because each member has an equal chance of inclusion.

With very large samples, estimates become quite reliable. E is estimated to represent 12.7 percent of the letters in written English. E, in fact, is 12.3 percent of the 925,141 letters in Melville’s Moby Dick, 12.4 percent of the 586,747 letters in Dickens’ A Tale of Two Cities, and 12.1 percent of the 3,901,021 letters in 12 of Mark Twain’s works (Chance News, 1997).

Political pollsters sample voters in national election surveys just this way. Using only 1500 randomly sampled people, drawn from all areas of a country, they can provide a remarkably accurate snapshot of the nation’s opinions. Without random sampling, large samples—including call-in phone samples and TV or website polls—often merely give misleading results.

The point to remember: Before accepting survey findings, think critically: Consider the sample. The best basis for generalizing is from a representative sample. You cannot compensate for an unrepresentative sample by simply adding more people.

 

Correlation

1-9: What are positive and negative correlations, and why do they enable prediction but not cause-effect explanation?

Describing behavior is a first step toward predicting it. Naturalistic observations and surveys often show us that one trait or behavior is related to another. In such cases, we say the two correlate. A statistical measure (the correlation coefficient) indicates how closely two things vary together, and thus how well either one predicts the other. Knowing how much aptitude test scores correlate with school success tells us how well the scores predict school success.

correlation a measure of the extent to which two factors vary together, and thus of how well either factor predicts the other.

correlation coefficient a statistical index of the relationship between two things (from −1 to +1).

positive correlation (between 0 and +1.00) indicates a direct relationship, meaning that two things increase together or decrease together.

negative correlation (between 0 and −1.00) indicates an inverse relationship: As one thing increases, the other decreases. The weekly number of hours spent in TV watching and video gaming correlates negatively with grades. Negative correlations could go as low as −1.00, which means that, like people on the opposite ends of a teeter-totter, one set of scores goes down precisely as the other goes up.

Though informative, psychology’s correlations usually leave most of the variation among individuals unpredicted. As we will see, there is a positive correlation between parents’ abusiveness and their children’s later abusiveness when they become parents. But this does not mean that most abused children become abusive. The correlation simply indicates a statistical relationship: Most abused children do not grow into abusers, but nonabused children are even less likely to become abusive. Correlations point us toward predictions, but usually imperfect ones.

The point to remember: A correlation coefficient helps us see the world more clearly by revealing the extent to which two things relate.

 

Correlation and Causation

Correlations help us predict. The New York Times reports that U.S. counties with high gun ownership rates tend to have high murder rates (Luo, 2011). Gun ownership predicts homicide. What might explain this guns-homicide correlation?

I can almost hear someone thinking, “Well, of course, guns kill people, often in moments of passion.” If so, that could be an example of A (guns) causes B (murder). But I can hear other readers saying, “Not so fast. Maybe people in dangerous places buy more guns for self-protection—maybe B causes A.” Or maybe some third factor C causes both A and B.

Another example: Self-esteem correlates negatively with (and therefore predicts) depression. (The lower people’s self-esteem, the more they are at risk for depression.) So, does low self-esteem causedepression? If, based on the correlational evidence, you assume that it does, you have much company. A nearly irresistible thinking error is assuming that an association, sometimes presented as a correlation coefficient, proves causation. But no matter how strong the relationship, it does not.

This point is so important—so basic to thinking smarter with psychology—that it merits one more example. A survey of over 12,000 adolescents found that the more teens feel loved by their parents, the less likely they are to behave in unhealthy ways—having early sex, smoking, abusing alcohol and drugs, exhibiting violence (Resnick et al., 1997). “Adults have a powerful effect on their children’s behavior right through the high school years,” gushed an Associated Press (AP) story reporting the finding. But this correlation comes with no built-in cause-effect arrow. The AP could as well have reported, “Well-behaved teens feel their parents’ love and approval; out-of-bounds teens more often think their parents are disapproving.”

The point to remember (turn the volume up here): Association does not prove causation. Correlation indicates the possibility of a cause-effect relationship but does not prove such. Remember this principle and you will be wiser as you read and hear news of scientific studies.

New York Times writer reported a massive survey showing that “adolescents whose parents smoked were 50 percent more likely than children of nonsmokers to report having had sex.” He concluded (would you agree?) that the survey indicated a causal effect—that “to reduce the chances that their children will become sexually active at an early age” parents might “quit smoking” (O’Neil, 2002).

Experimentation

1-10: What are the characteristics of experimentation that make it possible to isolate cause and effect?

Happy are they, remarked the Roman poet Virgil, “who have been able to perceive the causes of things.” How might psychologists perceive causes in correlational studies, such as the correlation between breast feeding and intelligence?

Researchers have found that the intelligence scores of children who were breast-fed as infants are somewhat higher than the scores of children who were bottle-fed with cow’s milk (Angelsen et al., 2001; Mortensen et al., 2002; Quinn et al., 2001). In Britain, breast-fed babies have also been more likely than their bottle-fed counterparts to eventually move into a higher social class (Martin et al., 2007). But the “breast is best” intelligence effect shrinks when researchers compare breast-fed and bottle-fed children from the same families (Der et al., 2006).

What do such findings mean? Do smarter mothers (who in modern countries more often breast feed) have smarter children? Or, as some researchers believe, do the nutrients of mother’s milk contribute to brain development? To find answers to such questions—to isolate cause and effect—researchers can  experiment . Experiments enable researchers to isolate the effects of one or more factors by (1) manipulating the factors of interest and (2) holding constant (controlling) other factors. To do so, they often create an  experimental group , in which people receive the treatment, and a contrasting  control group  whose members do not receive the treatment. To minimize any preexisting differences between the two groups, researchers randomly assign people to the two conditions. If one-third of the volunteers for an experiment can wiggle their ears, then about one-third of the people in each group will be ear wigglers. So, too, with ages, attitudes, and other characteristics, which will be similar in the experimental and control groups. Thus, if the groups differ at the experiment’s end, we can surmise that the treatment had an effect.

experiment a research method in which an investigator manipulates one or more factors (independent variables) to observe the effect on some behavior or mental process (the dependent variable). By random assignment of participants, the experimenter aims to control other relevant factors.

experimental group in an experiment, the group exposed to the treatment, that is, to one version of the independent variable.

control group in an experiment, the group not exposed to the treatment; contrasts with the experimental group and serves as a comparison for evaluating the effect of the treatment.

random assignment assigning participants to experimental and control groups by chance, thus minimizing preexisting differences between the different groups.

To experiment with breast feeding, one research team randomly assigned some 17,000 Belarus newborns and their mothers either to a breast-feeding-promotion group or to a normal pediatric care program (Kramer et al., 2008). At three months of age, 43 percent of the experimental group infants were being exclusively breast-fed, as were 6 percent in the control group. At age 6, when nearly 14,000 of the children were restudied, those who had been in the breast-feeding-promotion group had intelligence test scores averaging six points higher than their control group counterparts.

No single experiment is conclusive, of course. But randomly assigning participants to one feeding group or the other effectively eliminated all factors except nutrition. This supported the conclusion that breast is indeed best for developing intelligence: If a behavior (such as test performance) changes when we vary an experimental factor (such as infant nutrition), then we infer that the factor is having an effect.

The point to remember: Unlike correlational studies, which uncover naturally occurring relationships, an experiment manipulates a factor to determine its effect.

Consider, then, how we might assess therapeutic interventions. Our tendency to seek new remedies when we are ill or emotionally down can produce misleading testimonies. If three days into a cold we start taking vitamin C tablets and find our cold symptoms lessening, we may credit the pills rather than the cold naturally subsiding. In the 1700s, bloodletting seemed effective. People sometimes improved after the treatment; when they didn’t, the practitioner inferred the disease was too advanced to be reversed. So, whether or not a remedy is truly effective, enthusiastic users will probably endorse it. To determine its effect, we must control for other factors.

And that is precisely how investigators evaluate new drug treatments and new methods of psychological therapy. They randomly assign participants either to the group receiving a treatment (such as a medication), or to a group receiving a pseudo-treatment—an inert placebo(perhaps a pill with no drug in it). The participants are often blind (uninformed) about what treatment, if any, they are receiving. If the study is using a  double-blind procedure , neither the participants nor those who administer the drug or placebo and collect the data will know which group is receiving the treatment.

double-blind procedure an experimental procedure in which both the research participants and the research staff are ignorant (blind) about whether the research participants have received the treatment or a placebo. Commonly used in drug-evaluation studies.

In such studies, researchers can check a treatment’s actual effects apart from the participants’ and the staff’s belief in its healing powers. Just thinking you are getting a treatment can boost your spirits, relax your body, and relieve your symptoms. This  placebo effect  is well documented in reducing pain, depression, and anxiety (Kirsch, 2010). And the more expensive the placebo, the more “real” it seems to us—a fake pill that costs $2.50 works better than one costing 10 cents (Waber et al., 2008). To know how effective a therapy really is, researchers must control for a possible placebo effect.

placebo [pluh-SEE-bo; Latin for “I shall please”] ffect experimental results caused by expectations alone; any effect on behavior caused by the administration of an inert substance or condition, which the recipient assumes is an active agent.

 

Independent and Dependent Variables

Here is an even more potent example: The drug Viagra was approved for use after 21 clinical trials. One trial was an experiment in which researchers randomly assigned 329 men with erectile disorder to either an experimental group (Viagra takers) or a control group (placebo takers). It was a double-blind procedure—neither the men nor the person giving them the pills knew what they were receiving. The result: At peak doses, 69 percent of Viagra-assisted attempts at intercourse were successful, compared with 22 percent for men receiving the placebo (Goldstein et al., 1998). Viagra worked.

This simple experiment manipulated just one factor: the drug dosage (none versus peak dose). We call this experimental factor the independent variable because we can vary it independently of other factors, such as the men’s age, weight, and personality. These other factors, which could influence the experiment’s results, are called confounding variables. Random assignment controls for possible confounding variables.

independent variable the experimental factor that is manipulated; the variable whose effect is being studied.

confounding variable a factor other than the independent variable that might produce an effect in an experiment.

Experiments examine the effect of one or more independent variables on some measurable behavior, called the dependent variable because it can vary depending on what takes place during the experiment. Both variables are given precise operational definitions, which specify the procedures that manipulate the independent variable (in this study, the precise drug dosage and timing) or measure the dependent variable (the questions that assessed the men’s responses). These definitions answer the “What do you mean?” question with a level of precision that enables others to repeat the study.

dependent variable the outcome factor; the variable that may change in response to manipulations of the independent variable.

 

Let’s pause to check your understanding using a simple psychology experiment: To test the effect of perceived ethnicity on the availability of rental housing, Adrian Carpusor and William Loges (2006) sent identically worded e-mail inquiries to 1115 Los Angeles–area landlords. The researchers varied the ethnic connotation of the sender’s name and tracked the percentage of positive replies (invitations to view the apartment in person). “Patrick McDougall,” “Said Al-Rahman,” and “Tyrell Jackson” received, respectively, 89 percent, 66 percent, and 56 percent invitations.

Experiments can also help us evaluate social programs. Do early childhood education programs boost impoverished children’s chances for success? What are the effects of different anti-smoking campaigns? Do school sex-education programs reduce teen pregnancies? To answer such questions, we can experiment: If an intervention is welcomed but resources are scarce, we could use a lottery to randomly assign some people (or regions) to experience the new program and others to a control condition. If later the two groups differ, the intervention’s effect will be confirmed (Passell, 1993).

Let’s recap. A variable is anything that can vary (infant nutrition, intelligence, TV exposure—anything within the bounds of what is feasible and ethical). Experiments aim to manipulate an independent variable, measure the dependent variable, and control confounding variables. An experiment has at least two different conditions: an experimental condition and a comparison or control conditionRandom assignment works to minimize preexisting differences between the groups before any treatment effects occur. In this way, an experiment tests the effect of at least one independent variable (what we manipulate) on at least one dependent variable (the outcome we measure). TABLE 1.2 compares the features of psychology’s research methods.

 

Table 1.2: Comparing Research Methods

Research Method Basic Purpose How Conducted What Is Manipulated Weaknesses
Descriptive To observe and record behavior Do case studies, naturalistic observations, or surveys Nothing No control of variables; single cases may be misleading
Correlational To detect naturally occurring relationships; to assess how well one variable predicts another Collect data on two or more variables; no manipulation Nothing Does not specify cause and effect
Experimental To explore cause and effect Manipulate one or more factors; use random assignment The independent variable(s) Sometimes not feasible; results may not generalize to other contexts; not ethical to manipulate certain variables

 

Frequently Asked Questions About Psychology

We have reflected on how a scientific approach can restrain biases. We have seen how case studies, naturalistic observations, and surveys help us describe behavior. We have also noted that correlational studies assess the association between two factors, which indicates how well one thing predicts another. We have examined the logic that underlies experiments, which use control conditions and random assignment of participants to isolate the effects of an independent variable on a dependent variable.

Yet, even knowing this much, you may still be approaching psychology with a mixture of curiosity and apprehension. So before we plunge in, let’s entertain some frequently asked questions.

1-11: Can laboratory experiments illuminate everyday life?

When you see or hear about psychological research, do you ever wonder whether people’s behavior in the lab will predict their behavior in real life? For example, does detecting the blink of a faint red light in a dark room reveal anything useful about flying a plane at night? After viewing a violent, sexually explicit film, does an aroused man’s increased willingness to push buttons that he thinks will electrically shock a woman really say anything about whether violent pornography makes a man more likely to abuse a woman?

Before you answer, consider: The experimenter intends the laboratory environment to be a simplified reality—one that simulates and controls important features of everyday life. Just as a wind tunnel lets airplane designers re-create airflow forces under controlled conditions, a laboratory experiment lets psychologists re-create psychological forces under controlled conditions.

An experiment’s purpose is not to re-create the exact behaviors of everyday life but to test theoretical principles (Mook, 1983). In aggression studies, deciding whether to push a button that delivers a shock may not be the same as slapping someone in the face, but the principle is the same. It is the resulting principles—not the specific findings—that help explain everyday behaviors.

When psychologists apply laboratory research on aggression to actual violence, they are applying theoretical principles of aggressive behavior, principles refined through many experiments. Similarly, it is the principles of the visual system, developed from experiments in artificial settings (such as looking at red lights in the dark), that researchers apply to more complex behaviors such as night flying. And many investigations have demonstrated that principles derived in the laboratory do typically generalize to the everyday world (Anderson et al., 1999).

The point to remember: Psychological science focuses less on particular behaviors than on seeking general principles that help explain many behaviors.

1-12: Does behavior depend on one’s culture and gender?

India is not Indiana

What can we learn about people in general from psychological studies done in one time and place—often with people from what Joseph Henrich, Steven Heine, and Ara Norenzayan (2010) call the WEIRD cultures (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic cultures that contribute most study participants but are only 12 percent of humanity)? As we will see time and again, culture—shared ideas and behaviors that one generation passes on to the next—matters. Our culture shapes our standards of promptness and frankness, our attitudes toward premarital sex and varying body shapes, our tendency to be casual or formal, our willingness to make eye contact, our conversational distance, and much, much more. Being aware of such differences, we can restrain our assumptions that others will think and act as we do. Given the growing mixing and clashing of cultures, our need for such awareness is urgent.

culture the enduring behaviors, ideas, attitudes, values, and traditions shared by a group of people and transmitted from one generation to the next.

It is also true, however, that our shared biological heritage unites us as a universal human family. The same underlying processes guide people everywhere:

·  People diagnosed with the specific learning disorder (formerly called dyslexia), exhibit the same brain malfunction whether they are Italian, French, or British (Paulesu et al., 2001).

·  Variation in languages may impede communication across cultures. Yet all languages share deep principles of grammar, and people from opposite hemispheres can communicate with a smile or a frown.

·  People in different cultures vary in feelings of loneliness. But across cultures, loneliness is magnified by shyness, low self-esteem, and being unmarried (Jones et al., 1985; Rokach et al., 2002).

We are each in certain respects like all others, like some others, and like no other. Studying people of all races and cultures helps us discern our similarities and our differences, our human kinship and our diversity.

You will see throughout this book that gender matters, too. Researchers report gender differences in what we dream, in how we express and detect emotions, and in our risk for alcohol use disorder, depression, and eating disorders. Gender differences fascinate us, and studying them is potentially beneficial. For example, many researchers believe that women carry on conversations more readily to build relationships, while men talk more to give information and advice (Tannen, 2001). Knowing this difference can help us prevent conflicts and misunderstandings in everyday relationships.

But again, psychologically as well as biologically, women and men are overwhelmingly similar. Whether female or male, we learn to walk at about the same age. We experience the same sensations of light and sound. We feel the same pangs of hunger, desire, and fear. We exhibit similar overall intelligence and well-being.

The point to remember: Even when specific attitudes and behaviors vary by gender or across cultures, as they often do, the underlying processes are much the same.

“All people are the same; only their habits differ.”

Confucius, 551–479 B.C.E

1-13: Why do psychologists study animals, and what ethical guidelines safeguard human and animal research participants?

Many psychologists study animals because they find them fascinating. They want to understand how different species learn, think, and behave. Psychologists also study animals to learn about people. We humans are not like animals; we are animals, sharing a common biology. Animal experiments have therefore led to treatments for human diseases—insulin for diabetes, vaccines to prevent polio and rabies, transplants to replace defective organs.

“Rats are very similar to humans except that they are not stupid enough to purchase lottery tickets.”

Dave Barry, July 2, 2002

Humans are more complex, but the same processes by which we learn are present in rats, monkeys, and even sea slugs. The simplicity of the sea slug’s nervous system is precisely what makes it so revealing of the neural mechanisms of learning.

Sharing such similarities, should we respect rather than experiment on our animal relatives? The animal protection movement protests the use of animals in psychological, biological, and medical research.

“Please do not forget those of us who suffer from incurable diseases or disabilities who hope for a cure through research that requires the use of animals.”

Psychologist Dennis Feeney (1987)

Out of this heated debate, two issues emerge. The basic one is whether it is right to place the well-being of humans above that of animals. In experiments on stress and cancer, is it right that mice get tumors in the hope that people might not? Should some monkeys be exposed to an HIV-like virus in the search for an AIDS vaccine? Is our use and consumption of other animals as natural as the behavior of carnivorous hawks, cats, and whales? The answers to such questions vary by culture. In Gallup surveys in Canada and the United States, about 60 percent of adults have deemed medical testing on animals “morally acceptable.” In Britain, only 37 percent have (Mason, 2003).

“The greatness of a nation can be judged by the way its animals are treated.”

Mahatma Gandhi, 1869–1948

If we give human life first priority, what safeguards should protect the well-being of animals in research? In one survey of animal researchers, 98 percent supported government regulations protecting primates, dogs, and cats, and 74 percent supported regulations providing for the humane care of rats and mice (Plous & Herzog, 2000). Many professional associations and funding agencies already have such guidelines. British Psychological Society guidelines call for housing animals under reasonably natural living conditions, with companions for social animals (Lea, 2000). American Psychological Association (APA) guidelines state that researchers must ensure the “comfort, health, and humane treatment” of animals and minimize “infection, illness, and pain” (APA, 2002). The European Parliament now mandates standards for animal care and housing (Vogel, 2010).

Animals have themselves benefited from animal research. One Ohio team of research psychologists measured stress hormone levels in samples of millions of dogs brought each year to animal shelters. They devised handling and stroking methods to reduce stress and ease the dogs’ transition to adoptive homes (Tuber et al., 1999). Other studies have helped improve care and management in animals’ natural habitats. By revealing our behavioral kinship with animals and the remarkable intelligence of chimpanzees, gorillas, and other animals, experiments have also led to increased empathy and protection for them. At its best, a psychology concerned for humans and sensitive to animals serves the welfare of both.

Animal research benefiting animals

What about human participants? Does the image of white-coated scientists delivering electric shocks trouble you? If so, you’ll be relieved to know that most psychological studies are free of such stress. With people, blinking lights, flashing words, and pleasant social interactions are more common. Moreover, psychology’s experiments are mild compared with the stress and humiliation often inflicted by reality TV shows. In one episode of The Bachelor, a man dumped his new fiancée—on camera, at the producers’ request—for the woman who earlier had finished second (Collins, 2009).

Occasionally, though, researchers do temporarily stress or deceive people, but only when they believe it is essential to a justifiable end, such as understanding and controlling violent behavior or studying mood swings. Some experiments won’t work if participants know everything beforehand. (Wanting to be helpful, the participants might try to confirm the researcher’s predictions.)

The APA ethics code urges researchers to (1) obtain human participants’ informed consent before the experiment, (2) protect them from harm and discomfort, (3) keep information about individual participants confidential, and (4) fully debrief people (explain the research afterward). Moreover, university ethics committees screen research proposals and safeguard participants’ well-being.

informed consent giving potential participants enough information about a study to enable them to decide whether they wish to participate.

debriefing the postexperimental explanation of a study, including its purpose and any deceptions, to its participants.

1-14: Is psychology free of value judgments?

Psychology is definitely not value free. Values affect what we study, how we study it, and how we interpret results. Researchers’ values influence their choice of topics. Should we study worker productivity or worker morale? Sex discrimination or gender differences? Conformity or independence? Values can also color “the facts.” As we noted earlier, our preconceptions can bias our observations and interpretations; sometimes we see what we want or expect to see

Even the words we use to describe something can reflect our values. Are the sex acts we do not practice “perversions” or “sexual variations”? In psychology and in everyday speech, labels describe and labels evaluate: One person’s rigidity is another’s consistency. One person’s faith is another’s fanaticism. One country’s enhanced interrogation techniques, such as cold-water immersion, become torture when practiced by its enemies. Our labeling someone as firm or stubborncareful or pickydiscreet or secretive reveals our own attitudes.

Popular applications of psychology also contain hidden values. If you defer to “professional” guidance about how to live—how to raise children, how to achieve self-fulfillment, what to do with sexual feelings, how to get ahead at work—you are accepting value-laden advice. A science of behavior and mental processes can help us reach our goals. But it cannot decide what those goals should be.

Knowledge transforms us. Learning about the solar system and the germ theory of disease alters the way people think and act. Learning about psychology’s findings also changes people: They less often judge psychological disorders as moral failings, treatable by punishment and ostracism. They less often regard and treat women as men’s mental inferiors. They less often view and rear children as ignorant, willful beasts in need of taming. “In each case,” noted Morton Hunt (1990, p. 206), “knowledge has modified attitudes, and, through them, behavior.” Once aware of psychology’s well-researched ideas—about how body and mind connect, how a child’s mind grows, how we construct our perceptions, how we remember (and misremember) our experiences, how people across the world differ (and are alike)—your mind may never again be quite the same.

But bear in mind psychology’s limits. Don’t expect it to answer the ultimate questions, such as those posed by Russian novelist Leo Tolstoy (1904): “Why should I live? Why should I do anything? Is there in life any purpose which the inevitable death that awaits me does not undo and destroy?”

Although many of life’s significant questions are beyond psychology, some very important ones are illuminated by even a first psychology course. Through painstaking research, psychologists have gained insights into brain and mind, dreams and memories, depression and joy. Even the unanswered questions can renew our sense of mystery about “things too wonderful” for us yet to understand. Moreover, your study of psychology can help teach you how to ask and answer important questions—how to think critically as you evaluate competing ideas and claims.

Psychology speaks

If some people see psychology as merely common sense, others have a different concern—that it is becoming dangerously powerful. Is it an accident that astronomy is the oldest science and psychology the youngest? To some, exploring the external universe seems far safer than exploring our own inner universe. Might psychology, they ask, be used to manipulate people?

Knowledge, like all power, can be used for good or evil. Nuclear power has been used to light up cities—and to demolish them. Persuasive power has been used to educate people—and to deceive them. Although psychology does indeed have the power to deceive, its purpose is to enlighten. Every day, psychologists are exploring ways to enhance learning, creativity, and compassion. Psychology speaks to many of our world’s great problems—war, overpopulation, prejudice, family crises, crime—all of which involve attitudes and behaviors. Psychology also speaks to our deepest longings—for nourishment, for love, for happiness. Psychology cannot address all of life’s great questions, but it speaks to some mighty important ones.

Improve Your Retention–and Your Grades

1-15: How can psychological principles help you learn and remember?

Do you, like most students, assume that the way to cement your new learning is to reread? What helps even more—and what this book therefore encourages—is repeated self-testing and rehearsal of previously studied material. Memory researchers Henry Roediger and Jeffrey Karpicke (2006) call this phenomenon the  testing effect . (It is also sometimes called the retrieval practice effect or test-enhanced learning.) They note that “testing is a powerful means of improving learning, not just assessing it.” In one of their studies, students recalled the meaning of 40 previously learned Swahili words much better if tested repeatedly than if they spent the same time restudying the words (Karpicke & Roediger, 2008).

testing effect enhanced memory after retrieving, rather than simply rereading, information. Also sometimes referred to as a retrieval practice effect or test-enhanced learning.

As you will see in Chapter 8, to master information you must actively process it. Your mind is not like your stomach, something to be filled passively; it is more like a muscle that grows stronger with exercise. Countless experiments reveal that people learn and remember best when they put material in their own words, rehearse it, and then retrieve and review it again.

The SQ3R study method incorporates these principles (McDaniel et al., 2009; Robinson, 1970). SQ3R is an acronym for its five steps: Survey, Question, Read, Retrieve, Review.

SQ3R a study method incorporating five steps: Survey, Question, Read, Retrieve, Review.

To study a chapter, first survey, taking a bird’s-eye view. Scan the headings, and notice how the chapter is organized.

Before you read each main section, try to answer its numbered Learning Objective Question (for this section: “How can psychological principles help you learn and remember?”). Roediger and Bridgid Finn (2009) have found that “trying and failing to retrieve the answer is actually helpful to learning.” Those who test their understanding before reading, and discover what they don’t yet know, will learn and remember better.

Then read, actively searching for the answer to the question. At each sitting, read only as much of the chapter (usually a single main section) as you can absorb without tiring. Read actively and critically. Ask questions. Take notes. Make the ideas your own: How does what you’ve read relate to your own life? Does it support or challenge your assumptions? How convincing is the evidence?

Having read a section, retrieve its main ideas. Test yourself. This will help you figure out what you know. Moreover, the testing itself will help you learn and retain the information more effectively. Even better, test yourself repeatedly. To facilitate this, I offer periodic Retrieve It opportunities throughout each chapter (see, for example, the questions on pages 29 and 31). After trying to answer these questions yourself, you can check the inverted answers, and reread as needed.

Finally, review: Read over any notes you have taken, again with an eye on the chapter’s organization, and quickly review the whole chapter. Write or say what a concept is before rereading to check your understanding.

Survey, question, read, retrieve, review. I have organized this book’s chapters to facilitate your use of the SQ3R study system. Each chapter begins with a chapter outline that aids your survey. Headings and Learning Objective Questions suggest issues and concepts you should consider as you read. The material is organized into sections of readable length. The Retrieve It questions will challenge you to retrieve what you have learned, and thus better remember it. The end-of-chapter Review provides more opportunities for active processing and self-testing, focusing on the chapter’s key terms and Learning Objective Questions.

 

Four additional study tips may further boost your learning:

Distribute your study time. One of psychology’s oldest findings is that spaced practice promotes better retention than does massed practice. You’ll remember material better if you space your practice time over several study periods—perhaps one hour a day, six days a week—rather than cram it into one long study blitz. For example, rather than trying to read an entire chapter in a single sitting, read just one main section and then turn to something else. Interleaving your study of psychology with your study of other subjects will boost your long-term retention and will protect against overconfidence (Kornell & Bjork, 2008; Taylor & Rohrer, 2010).

Spacing your study sessions requires a disciplined approach to managing your time. (Richard O. Straub explains time management in a helpful preface at the beginning of this text.)

Learn to think critically. Whether you are reading or in class, note people’s assumptions and values. What perspective or bias underlies an argument? Evaluate evidence. Is it anecdotal? Or is it based on informative experiments? Assess conclusions. Are there alternative explanations?

Process class information actively. Listen for a lecture’s main ideas and sub-ideas. Write them down. Ask questions during and after class. In class, as in your private study, process the information actively and you will understand and retain it better. As psychologist William James urged a century ago, “No reception without reaction, no impression without…expression.” Make the information your own. Take notes in your own words. Relate what you read to what you already know. Tell someone else about it. (As any teacher will confirm, to teach is to remember.)

Overlearn. Psychology tells us that overlearning improves retention. We are prone to overestimating how much we know. You may understand a chapter as you read it, but that feeling of familiarity can be deceptively comforting. Using the Retrieve It opportunities, devote extra study time to testing your knowledge.

Memory experts Elizabeth Bjork and Robert Bjork (2011, p. 63) offer the bottom line for how to improve your retention and your grades:

Spend less time on the input side and more time on the output side, such as summarizing what you have read from memory or getting together with friends and asking each other questions. Any activities that involve testing yourself—that is, activities that require you to retrieve or generate information, rather than just representing information to yourself—will make your learning both more durable and flexible.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Discussion: Attention And Consciousness

Discussion: Attention And Consciousness

Attention is a cognitive function that plays a central role in almost everything we do throughout the day. Based on this week’s readings, discuss the area/aspect of attention that you think is most important for or most influential on cognition. For example, when is vigilance or divided attention important for cognitive processing? Or how can unconscious processing influence our perception of an item? Be sure to support your opinions with specific terms and theories from the readings. How could you apply the ideas you have discussed to different settings, for example, in driving or advertising?

please follow and support the questions in detail.

No Grammar issue , No Plag APA format.

I will add a student paper under here

Student Answer below do not copy rewrite your own answer

Student Answer

 

Attention is choosing and processing a specific amount of information from all of the information that is being absorbed by our senses, stored memories and other cognitive processes (Sternberg & Sternberg, 2017, p. 119). The four main areas of attention are signal detection and vigilance, search, selective attention and divided attention. Although they are each very important depending on the circumstances, selective attentions stands out to be the most important for cognition. For cognitive processes to happen, a person has to be able to pay attention to and focus on something for a certain amount of time.

There are several “filter and bottleneck” theories to help explain selective attention. A filter blocks some of the information going through, selecting only a part of it while the bottleneck slows down the information that is passing through (Sternberg & Sternberg, 2017, p. 128). This can be explained more with the Early Filter Model by Donald Broadbent which says that all information comes through and is stored in sensory memory for a very short period of time. It is then moved forward to a filter that will only allow one message to move forward again and be processed in more detail. From our short-term memory, we can pick and choose which information we want to keep and use later.

This is important for cognitive processing because if we were not able to selectively focus on certain information while other conversations around us are happening, it would be too hard to process the information and store it for later use. A weakness of this is that sometimes the information that is picked up on is due to the loudness, pitch, accent (Sternberg & Sternberg, 2017) or sometimes whatever information sounds more exciting versus what is more important. This is known as the cocktail party problem. For those who are not very good with divided attention, selective attention is very important when driving past an accident. At the same time, divided attention is more important for drivers than selective because there is always a lot to look out for while driving.

References

Sternberg, R. J., & Sternberg, K. (2016). Cognitive Psychology (7th ed.) Boston: MA. Cengage Learning. P. 117-154.

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!

Finding Proportions

Finding Proportions

Chapter 5, numbers 5.11, 5.13, 5.15, and 5.18

Finding Proportions

5.11 Scores on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) approximate a normal curve with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. What proportion of IQ scores are

(a) above Kristen’s 125?

(b) below 82?

(c) within 9 points of the mean?

(d) more than 40 points from the mean?

 

Finding Scores

5.13 IQ scores on the WAIS test approximate a normal curve with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. What IQ score is identified with

(a) the upper 2 percent, that is, 2 percent to the right (and 98 percent to the left)?

(b) the lower 10 percent?

(c) the upper 60 percent?

(d) the middle 95 percent? [Remember, the middle 95 percent straddles the line perpendicular to the mean (or the 50th percentile), with half of 95 percent, or 47.5 percent, above this line and the remaining 47.5 percent below this line.]

(e) the middle 99 percent?

Finding Proportions and Scores IMPORTANT NOTE: When doing Questions 5.15 and 5.16, remember to decide first whether a proportion or a score is to be found.

*5. 15 An investigator polls common cold sufferer, asking them to estimate the number of hours of physical discomfort caused by their most recent colds. Assume that their estimates approximate a normal curve with a mean of 83 hours and a standard deviation of 20 hours.

(a) What is the estimated number of hours for the shortest-suffering 5 percent?

(b) What proportion of sufferers estimate that their colds lasted longer than 48 hours?

(c) What proportion suffered for fewer than 61 hours?

(d) What is the estimated number of hours suffered by the extreme 1 percent either above or below the mean?

(e) What proportion suffered for between 1 and 3 days, that is, between 24 and 72 hours?

(f) What is the estimated number of hours suffered by the middle 95 percent? [See the comment about “middle 95 percent” in Question 5.13(d).]

(g) What proportion suffered for between 2 and 4 days?

(h) A medical researcher wishes to concentrate on the 20 percent who suffered the most. She will work only with those who estimate that they suffered for more than ——— hours.

(I) Another researcher wishes to compare those who suffered least with those who suffered most. If each group is to consist of only the extreme 3 percent, the mild group will consist of those who suffered for fewer than _____ hours, and the severe group will consist of those who suffered for more than _____ hours.

(j) Another survey found that people with colds who took daily doses of vitamin C suf-fered, on the average, for 61 hours. What proportion of the original survey (with a mean of 83 hours and a standard deviation of 20 hours) suffered for more than 61 hours?

(k) What proportion of the original survey suffered for exactly 61 hours?

(Be careful!) Answers on page 427.

NORMAL DISTRIBUTIONS AND STANDARD (z) SCORES

*5.18 The body mass index (BMI) measures body size in people by dividing weight (in pounds) by the square of height (in inches) and then multiplying by a factor of 703. A BMI less than 18.5 is defined as underweight; between 18.5 to 24.9 is normal; between 25 and 29.9 is overweight; and 30 or more is obese. It is well established that Americans have become heavier during the last half century. Assume that the positively skewed distribution of BMIs for adult American males has a mean of 28 with a standard deviation of 4.

(a) Would the median BMI score exceed, equal, or be exceeded by the mean BMI score of 28?

Answer:

Mean exceeds median

(b) What z score defines overweight

Answer:

-0.75

(c) What z score defines obese. Answers on page 427.

0.50

Chapter 8, numbers 8.10, 8.14, 8.16, 8.19, and 8.21

 

8.10 Television stations sometimes solicit feedback volunteered by viewers about a tele-vised event. Following a televised debate between Barack Obama and Mitt Romney in the 2012 presidential election campaign, a TV station conducted a telephone poll to determine the “winner.” Callers were given two phone numbers, one for Obama and the other for Romney, to register their opinions automatically.

(a) Comment on whether this was a random sample.

(b) How might this poll have been improved?

*8.14 The probability of a boy being born equals .50, or 1/2, as does the probability of a girl being born. For a randomly selected family with two children, what’s the probability of

(a) two boys, that is, a boy and a boy? (Reminder: Before using either the addition or multiplication rule, satisfy yourself that the various events are either mutually exclusive or independent, respectively.)

Answer:

(b) two girls?

Answer:

(c) either two boys or two girls?

Answer:

Note: Answers on page 431.

 

8.16 A traditional test for extrasensory perception (ESP) involves a set of playing cards, each of which shows a different symbol (circle, square, cross, star, or wavy lines). If C represents a correct guess and I an incorrect guess, what is the probability of

(a) C?

(b) CI (in that order) for two guesses?

(c) CCC for three guesses?

(d) III for three guesses?

 

8.19 A sensor is used to monitor the performance of a nuclear reactor. The sensor accurately reflects the state of the reactor with a probability of .97. But with a probability of .02, it gives a false alarm (by reporting excessive radiation even though the reactor is performing normally), and with a probability of .01, it misses excessive radiation (by failing to report excessive radiation even though the reactor is performing abnormally).

 

(a) What is the probability that a sensor will give an incorrect report, that is, either a false alarm or a miss?

 

(b) To reduce costly shutdowns caused by false alarms, management introduces a second completely independent sensor, and the reactor is shut down only when both sensors report excessive radiation. (According to this perspective, solitary reports of excessive radiation should be viewed as false alarms and ignored, since both sensors provide accurate information much of the time.) What is the new probability that the reactor will be shut down because of simultaneous false alarms by both the first and second sensors?

 

(c) Being more concerned about failures to detect excessive radiation, someone who lives near the nuclear reactor proposes an entirely different strategy: Shut down the reactor whenever either sensor reports excessive radiation. (According to this point of view, even a solitary report of excessive radiation should trigger a shutdown, since a failure to detect excessive radiation is potentially catastrophic.) If this policy were adopted, what is the new probability that excessive radiation will be missed simultaneously by both the first and second sensors?

 

 

8.21 Assume that the probability of breast cancer equals .01 for women in the 50-59 age group. Furthermore, if a woman does have breast cancer, the probability of a true positive mammogram (correct detection of breast cancer) equals .80 and the probability of a false negative mammogram (a miss) equals .20. On the other hand, if a woman does not have breast cancer, the probability of a true negative mammogram (correct non-detection) equals .90 and the probability of a false positive mammogram (a false alarm) equals .10.

 

(a) What is the probability that a randomly selected woman will have a positive mammogram?

 

·

 

(b) What is the probability of having breast cancer, given a positive mammogram?

 

 

·

 

c) What is the probability of not having breast cancer, given a negative mammogram? Note: Answers on page 431.

·

(Hint: Use a frequency analysis to answer questions. To facilitate checking your answers with those in the book, begin with a total of 1,000 women, then branch into the number of women who do or do not have breast cancer, and finally, under each of these numbers, branch into the number of women with positive and negative mammograms.)

 
Do you need a similar assignment done for you from scratch? Order now!
Use Discount Code "Newclient" for a 15% Discount!